alexa Disambiguating the Relationship between Language Learning Anxiety (LLA) and Oral Performance: A Reconceptualization of Facilitative vs. Debilitative Anxieties | Open Access Journals
ISSN: 2378-5756
Journal of Psychiatry
Make the best use of Scientific Research and information from our 700+ peer reviewed, Open Access Journals that operates with the help of 50,000+ Editorial Board Members and esteemed reviewers and 1000+ Scientific associations in Medical, Clinical, Pharmaceutical, Engineering, Technology and Management Fields.
Meet Inspiring Speakers and Experts at our 3000+ Global Conferenceseries Events with over 600+ Conferences, 1200+ Symposiums and 1200+ Workshops on
Medical, Pharma, Engineering, Science, Technology and Business

Disambiguating the Relationship between Language Learning Anxiety (LLA) and Oral Performance: A Reconceptualization of Facilitative vs. Debilitative Anxieties

Mohammad Jahangiri1*, Azizah Binti Rajab2, Ali Jahangiri3, Ali Honarvar4 and Hamid Reza Sharif5

1Language Academy, Universiti Teknologi Malaysia (UTM) & Faculty of Humanities, IAUG, Azad University of Gachsaran, Gachsaran, Iran

2Language Academy, Universiti Teknologi Malaysia (UTM), Skudai, JB, Malaysia. Language Academy

3Shakhes Pajouh Engineering Research Institute, Esfahan University, Esfehan, Iran

4Faculty of Engineering, IAUG, Azad University of Gachsaran, Gachsaran, Iran

5Shiraz University, Shiraz, Iran

*Corresponding Author:
Mohammad Jahangiri
802/U8B, College Perdana
UTM, Skudai, JB, Malaysia, 81310
Tel: 0060127539701
E-mail: [email protected]

Received Date: August 14, 2014; Accepted Date: September 23, 2014; Published Date: September 30, 2014

Citation: Jahangiri M, Rajab AB, Jahangiri A, Honarvar A, Sharif HR (2014) Disambiguating the Relationship between Language Learning Anxiety (LLA) and Oral Performance: A Reconceptualization of Facilitative vs. Debilitative Anxieties. J Psychiatry 17:154. doi: 10.4172/Psychiatry.1000154

Copyright: © 2014 Mohammad Jahangiri et al., This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Visit for more related articles at Journal of Psychiatry

Keywords

Language Learning Anxiety (LLA); Anxiety Questionnaire (AQ); Oral Performance

Introduction

Anxiety has proved to be one of the most promising areas of research in English as a Foreign Language (EFL). Considerable research has shown that performance in a second language is related to measures of anxiety (Scovel, Phillips, Derakshan & Eysenck). In the foreign language classroom, high levels of anxiety are most likely to create a variety of negative effects. It is a popular belief that anxious students fail to manage classroom activities properly [1-3]. One of the many challenges in foreign language teaching is to provide such students with a low-anxiety classroom where they can possibly convey their ideas and feelings much better. As a result, a low-anxiety classroom would possibly promote learners' performance. In fact, there is research (e.g. Liu) submitting that large proportions of language students think of the foreign language class as anxiety-provoking suffering from alarming levels of debilitative anxiety. Most students find it very hard to stand before their peers presenting what they potentially know but fail to perform dynamically. Some even get deeply concerned when they feel they are being evaluated [4].

Although researchers think of anxiety as one of the main obstacles in foreign language oral performance, they are uncertain as to how to choose from among the various anxiety-removal strategies whereby a low–anxiety environment is created. More than that, there is no clear–cut relationship between anxiety and the Freudian Id, the Groddeck's it, and the unconscious in the associated literature [5]. This psychosomatic relationship between language learning and language production is clearly contemplated by Young who maintains that to study the language learning process is “to study how the body, mind, and emotions fuse to create self-expression". Elsewhere, Jahangiri, Rajab and Khosravi draw attention to the psychiatry of the hidden anxiety some university students experience and the role of teachers’ experiential knowledge in harnessing the potentially devastating debilitating anxiety and transforming this vast amount of detrimental energy to facilitating anxiety which promotes student performance.

Does language learning anxiety always exert a debilitating effect on oral performance? Alpert and Haber were among the first to suggest that this is not the case paving the way for a relativistic, as opposed to a unidimensionally-absolutistic, consideration of the term in future studies (Jahangiri et al.). [6,7]. Alpert and Haber subscribe to the view that anxiety may just as likely act as a facilitative factor. Therefore, it may be inferred that not all anxious individuals respond to a stressor in a similar way. In fact, some may think of a stressful situation as a challenge. Contrary to public opinion, anxiety might exert a positive effect on oral performance motivating the speaker for further or optimal practice, preparedness and performance in a highly-competitive environment such as the classroom. Thus, considering the predominantly-negative connotations associated with the term and the commonly-held views about the detrimental role of anxiety in oral performance, a heuristic data-driven reconsideration of the anxiety-performance relationship with no previous assumptions about the trends might be in order. Young examined the effect of anxiety on oral performance among prospective language teachers. The author was, before the completion of the study, of the opinion that anxiety would reduce scores on Oral Proficiency Interview. She, however, arrived at non significant correlations between the anxiety scores and those of the proficiency interview. She argues that ability is the main factor governing oral proficiency and that, after the acquisition of this ability, anxiety is of little effect. The above-mentioned comments reflect the widespread ambiguity in the literature, to say nothing of the more conflicting ones.

Stephenson examined the relationships between foreign language anxiety and global proficiency in English and between foreign language anxiety and performance on an oral test. The author (Stephenson) reports to have obtained a statistically significant negative relationship between language anxiety and oral test grades, and between language anxiety and two oral performance criteria and to have added new dimensions to the research on anxiety stating that foreign language anxiety is likely to “exert a deleterious influence” on both proficiency and oral performance while facilitating anxiety may improve oral performance “in moderately-anxious students. It is not difficult to imagine why public speaking, let alone speaking publicly in a foreign language, poses so many anxietal problems in ordinary people and students alike. Daly, cited in von Worde maintains that for some individuals fear of giving a public speech “exceeded such phobias as fear of snakes, elevators, and heights”. Horwitz et al. [3] consider “speaking anxiety” as “the most threatening” facet in FLL. Likewise, Price also subscribes to this view asserting that FL oral presentations were the most anxiety-provoking ordeal facing her research subjects. Young also found that the research subjects felt less anxious while speaking in small groups as opposed to the entire class.

To sum up and to apply these categorizations to our study, the present research intends to study the effect of language learning speaking anxiety on oral performance in the course of interviews. Thus, situational anxiety or situation-specific anxiety and output anxiety are mainly concerned here as language learning anxiety is generally categorized as a situation-specific anxiety and output anxiety is experienced during foreign language speaking which is the case in the present study.

Method

The scope of coverage of the present research is Language Learning Anxiety (LLA) as falling into two major categories Facilitative Anxiety and Debilitative Anxiety, henceforth referred to as FA and DA, respectively. The present study was performed at Azad University, Iran and the research subjects were selected from among EFL students enrolling in courses designed specifically to cater for their English communicative needs. The oral performance of the students was assessed with respect to their anxietal levels and types towards the end of the semester during which they received the intended treatments. The research topic is addressed with specific reference to two instruments: the Anxiety Questionnaire or the AQ, designed to investigate the severity and types of anxiety and the Stanford Foreign Language Oral Skills Evaluation Matrix acronymed FLOSEM (Padilla, Sung & Aninao, 1995) which measures oral performance [8].

Two groups of EFL students of the Azad University were subjected to two different treatments: Facilitative Anxiety and Debilitative Anxiety. This was undertaken to ensure that the subjects receive the diagonally-opposed ends of the continuum of the anxiety phenomenon to make it possible for the OLS to better represent the construct as opposed to a unidimensionally-negative view of anxiety which might bias the entire heuristic research process. Therefore, it was planned that the results from both treatments be merged for the purposes of this study, i.e. a general Ordinary Least Squares (OLS) regression line of best fit representative of the wide spectrum of the phenomenon. The students had enrolled in a course designed to teach English Speaking at a high level. The strategies for inducing the former are outlined in Jahangiri & Rajab [6]. Each group (n=50) was randomly selected from among students whose university language entry scores fitted the same category for which the English course was assigned. Subsequent to the treatments and towards the end of the term the researchers administered the AQ and the FLOSEM to gauge the gains/loss in oral proficiency marks in term of the widely different treatments the subjects received. The instruments were administered while students attended interviews. However, nothing was done to suggest the subjects of anything related to a research project. Nevertheless, consent was obtained about the fact that they were going to take part in a research project investigating foreign language learning. Maximal efforts were made to control for gender allowing almost an equal share of females and males in the same group. The university entrance exam had previously placed students in two groups of beginners (obtaining less than the 50 percent cutoff mark and the above-50-percent who may enroll for the highly communicative subject which was the focus of the study. All the subjects of this study fell into the category of intermediate and higher intermediate and advanced level students [9].

Results

This part is devoted to the representation of the results obtained from the two interviews. As was stated earlier, the two groups of subjects, the FA and the DA group were interviewed to assess their respective anxiety and oral performance levels and the relation there of Table 1 and 2 depict the descriptive statistics obtained for the variables studied for the two groups, together and in isolation, respectively.

Table 1: Descriptive for all the participants *Out of 125, **Out of 30

Table 2: Descriptive Statistics

As this study only aimed at the derivation of a general OLS line of best fit for the data, the data for the two groups were merged to better represent the wide spectrum of anxiety extremes and the continuum spanned by these extremes. As tables 1 and 2 suggest, the two groups, FA and DA, performed substantially differently subsequent to the treatments in terms of both the anxiety levels they experienced and the oral performance scores they obtained with the FA group obtaining a higher mean OP score and the DA group obtaining a higher mean anxiety score. To more optimally represent the extremes of the anxietal continuum, to allow consideration of the interaction effects, and to quantify the strength of the Anxiety-OP relationship Pearson correlations were obtained using the SPSS whose output is tabulated in Table 3.

Table 3: Correlations obtained between OP and Anxiety

Residuals and other relevant calculations were performed to derive a line of best fit for the data. It needs to be mentioned that a visual inspection of the scatter plot revealed a moderately curvilinear pattern which might account for the moderate Pearson coefficient obtained. This moderate coefficient may also be accounted for by the interplay and the interaction effects occurring between facilitative and debilitative anxieties.

An eyeball inspection of the scatter plot and the moderate correlation coefficient were indicative of the likelihood of a curvilinear pattern to be the case. As a result, appropriate software was used to subject the scatter plot to curves of best fit capturing the trend and pattern of the relationship. Figures 1-4 through 4 represent attempts at the exploration of this possibility.

psychiatry-anxiety-oral-performance

Figure 1: The anxiety-oral performance scatter plot

psychiatry-Linear-fit

Figure 2: Linear fit

psychiatry-Quadratic-fit

Figure 3: Quadratic fit Figure

psychiatry-4th-Degree-Polynomial-Fit

Figure 4: 4th Degree Polynomial Fit

As a first step towards capturing the variation of performance relative to levels of anxiety, the linear relationship was gauged deriving the OLS line of best fit for the above data. Tables 4 and 5 represent SPSS output produced for the OLS capturing the association.

Table 4: ANOVA, a. Predictors: (Constant), FA and DA Post Anx, b. Dependent Variable: FA and DA Post OP Which runs as follows OP=23.27-0.061AWhere OP represents Oral Performance as measured by FLOSEM and A represents anxiety (whether debilitative or facilitative).

Discussion

The tricky business of interpreting correlation coefficients may cause a lot of trouble and mistaken beliefs. In the first place, the coefficient indicates only the strength of a linear relationship. Second, correlation coefficients do not suggest causation. Pearson's correlation coefficient is widely used today in various fields including second language research. It ranges from -1.00 to + 1.00. When a positive correlation is obtained between two variables high quantities in one variable are connected with high quantities in another. In the case of a negative correlation, however, the increase in one variable is related to the decrease in another. More than that, a zero correlation implies that no relationship exists between the two variables. In simple words, the correlation coefficient, r, indicates the way in which two variables covary.

Consider the LLA-OP correlation obtained for the two groups merged, r=-0.56. SPSS produces the R Square value of 0.313 which can be employed to obtain the proportion of the variation of the Oral Performance (OP) due to its association with Language Learning Anxiety (LLA). In other words, 31 per cent of the variation of the oral production marks can be connected to (and not caused by) LLA [10-12].

Following the moderate correlation obtained and the somewhat curvilinear pattern revealed through a scatter plot of the data we come to the conclusion that the question of anxiety should not be discussed on absolute terms. In fact, the present paper might have indicated that what common people believe is unwarranted. In other words, increasing levels of anxiety might also be associated with improvements in oral performance. Despite some recent attempts at delineating facilitating anxiety and debilitating anxiety in the context of classrooms Jahangiri, Rajab, and Khosravi, the literature is yet to provide the readership with a clear-cut dividing line between the two constructs whose task might prove to be daunting for a number of reasons: Firstly, the two constructs coexist, collaborate and “transform” to each other, and the boundaries are blurred. Secondly, individuals might experience both, none, or some of one and some of the other at the same time and they might “work in tandem”. In fact, part of the reason why the researchers of the present study were not tempted to consider the separation (and hence the separate analyses) of the two FA and DA groups can be that the adoption of such a theoretical perspective, where FA and DA coexist, collaborate, interact, and transform into each other renders it impossible for them the two inalienable constructs. In other words, the adoption of this theoretical perspective signifies that any research undertaking that does not capture facilitating anxiety on a par with its debilitative counterpart cannot be taken seriously as it is limited in view of the lens it uses to observe a social phenomenon of such grandeur. Although correlation studies are not capable of implying causality, they can open the way for other avenues of research to proceed [13]. More than that, the present research is delimited by the statistical procedures themselves as these statistical inferences might not be conveniently transferred into pedagogical and practical inferences and policy making. In other words, statistical inferences might only suggest the directions along which educational policy making might proceed and what might be deemed by statisticians as statistically significant might not necessarily be considered by practitioners of the field and decision makers as reason to act on the basis of research findings. Put another way, statistical procedures should be viewed in conjunction with the realities governing the decision making process and the paraphernalia surrounding the issue in question. Taking all of these determining factors into consideration the authors are of the opinion that a substantial subdivision of the conflicting literature on anxiety can be summed up and disambiguated with specific reference to the following [14].

Although anxiety has most often been assumed to be following a linear pattern it is equally likely for anxiety to be a nonlinear phenomenon. This might explain why most of the related literature has led to conflicting and self-contradictory results [15,16].

The dividing line between debilitating and facilitating anxiety has not been clearly defined and the bulk of the literature mainly deals with debilitating anxiety following the popularly-held, simplistic and unsophisticated connotations implied by the word. Perhaps teachers should identify the anxiety groups prior to teaching and act accordingly, never trying to treat all the students the same way. That is, and they might reduce anxiety in some and produce anxiety among others.

While the researchers certainly admit the fact that the results emanating from the present study cannot be extended too far and that they are not claiming more for the relativistic consideration of the anxiety-performance relationship than it deserves they hope to have created an alternative vision of anxiety which might signal the directions along which future research on such a complex and intricate personality characteristic might proceed.

References

Select your language of interest to view the total content in your interested language
Post your comment

Share This Article

Relevant Topics

Recommended Conferences

Article Usage

  • Total views: 11739
  • [From(publication date):
    November-2014 - Oct 22, 2017]
  • Breakdown by view type
  • HTML page views : 7980
  • PDF downloads :3759
 

Post your comment

captcha   Reload  Can't read the image? click here to refresh

Peer Reviewed Journals
 
Make the best use of Scientific Research and information from our 700 + peer reviewed, Open Access Journals
International Conferences 2017-18
 
Meet Inspiring Speakers and Experts at our 3000+ Global Annual Meetings

Contact Us

Agri, Food, Aqua and Veterinary Science Journals

Dr. Krish

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001 Extn: 9040

Clinical and Biochemistry Journals

Datta A

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9037

Business & Management Journals

Ronald

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9042

Chemical Engineering and Chemistry Journals

Gabriel Shaw

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001 Extn: 9040

Earth & Environmental Sciences

Katie Wilson

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9042

Engineering Journals

James Franklin

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9042

General Science and Health care Journals

Andrea Jason

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9043

Genetics and Molecular Biology Journals

Anna Melissa

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001 Extn: 9006

Immunology & Microbiology Journals

David Gorantl

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9014

Informatics Journals

Stephanie Skinner

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9039

Material Sciences Journals

Rachle Green

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9039

Mathematics and Physics Journals

Jim Willison

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001 Extn: 9042

Medical Journals

Nimmi Anna

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001 Extn: 9038

Neuroscience & Psychology Journals

Nathan T

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9041

Pharmaceutical Sciences Journals

John Behannon

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9007

Social & Political Science Journals

Steve Harry

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001 Extn: 9042

 
© 2008-2017 OMICS International - Open Access Publisher. Best viewed in Mozilla Firefox | Google Chrome | Above IE 7.0 version
adwords