alexa Unpleasant Dream Content and Risk of Sleep Apnea | OMICS International
ISSN: 2378-5756
Journal of Psychiatry
Make the best use of Scientific Research and information from our 700+ peer reviewed, Open Access Journals that operates with the help of 50,000+ Editorial Board Members and esteemed reviewers and 1000+ Scientific associations in Medical, Clinical, Pharmaceutical, Engineering, Technology and Management Fields.
Meet Inspiring Speakers and Experts at our 3000+ Global Conferenceseries Events with over 600+ Conferences, 1200+ Symposiums and 1200+ Workshops on
Medical, Pharma, Engineering, Science, Technology and Business

Unpleasant Dream Content and Risk of Sleep Apnea

Habibolah Khazaie, Maryam Masoudi, Shahrzad Khoshbakht and Mohammad Rasoul Ghadami*

Sleep Disorders Research Center, Kermanshah University of Medical Sciences, Kermanshah, Iran

*Corresponding Author:
Mohammad Rasoul Ghadami
Sleep Disorders Research Center, Farabi Hospital
Kermanshah University of Medical Sciences
Kermanshah, Iran, PO Box: 6719851151
Tel: +98 9188332841
Fax: +98 (831)8264163
E-mail: [email protected]

Received Date: April 18, 2014; Accepted Date: July 28, 2014; Published Date: August 07, 2014

Citation: Khazaie H, Masoudi M, Khoshbakht S, Ghadami MR (2014) Unpleasant Dream Content and Risk of Sleep Apnea. J Psychiatry 17:136. doi: 10.4172/Psychiatry.1000136

Copyright: © 2014 Mohammad Rasoul Ghadami et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Visit for more related articles at Journal of Psychiatry

Keywords

Sleep apnea; Unpleasant dream content; Dream recall

Introduction

Dream is described as mental activity which happens during sleep [1]. It have been demonstrated that dreams reflect waking-life experiences such as visual or auditory perceptions, emotions and stress [2,3]. It is necessary to notice that dream itself cannot be evaluated straightly and the only available base is the dream report recalled by the person upon awakening.

Dreaming process may be affected by the sleep disorders [3]. Additionally, dream recall frequency has been related to sleep parameters such as rapid eye movement (REM) sleep physiology, frequency of nocturnal awakenings and sleep duration [3,4].
However, it's unclear that whether the presence of a sleep disorder like sleep apnea could affect dream content and recall.

Despite the several studies assessed some sleep problems such as insomnia and sleep apnea [5-8], most of them focused only on frequency of nightmare and dream recall; and dream content in sleep disorders is not clearly investigated.

The studies on the correlation between the sleep apnea and unpleasant dream were generally based on retrospective ascertainment of nightmare frequency, and there are controversies in their finding. Since, some small sample size studies reported that patients with obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) have negative emotional tone in dreams and dream emotion decreased with increasing apnea-hypopnea index (AHI) [9-12], others failed to confirm this and reported that nightmare frequency declines with Increasing AHI [6]. Because of disagreement between studies and lack of sufficient investigations on the study of risk of sleep apnea and unpleasant dream content, in this study we attempt to assess the relationship between risk of sleep apnea and unpleasant dream content in a large sample population.

Methods

During four months study period (September to December 2013), participants with ages between 20-65 years were selected using convenience sampling from Kermanshah in western Iran. Samples were selected from different areas of the city including offices, households and public places. The study was approved by the ethical committee of Kermanshah University of Medical Sciences and informed consent was obtained from all the participants. A brief screening with each individual with conversation of two expert psychologists who were responsible for data collection, showed no evidence of present or previous psychiatric disturbance in the subjects. Exclusion criteria were history of substance abuse, the presence of cardiovascular, respiratory, psychiatry, neurological and pregnancy. Also, participants with previous diagnosed sleep disorders (e.g., insomnia, periodic limb movement and sleep apnea) were excluded from the study. To evaluate the correlation between risk of sleep apnea and unpleasant dream content, at first, participants completed Berlin questionnaire (BQ) to assess their risk for sleep apnea. The BQ is a simple and useful screening tool for the prediction of sleep apnea risk in general population [13,14]. The BQ has 3 sections: In the first section, participants are asked to score their snoring. In section 2 daytime fatigue and sleepiness during day activities are asked; and in the last section, history of hypertension is asked and height and weight measured to calculated body mass index (BMI).The first two sections are assumed to be positive if the total score was two or more. If subject has hypertension or a BMI>30kg/m2, section 3 can be considered as positive. In general, if there are two or more sections with positive scores, this subject is categorized as "high risk” for sleep apnea. In the present study, we used their Persian version of BQ which was used and validated by Amra et al., with the sensitivity, specificity, positive and negative predictive values of 84.0%, 61.5%, 96.0%, 25.8%, respectively [14].

To evaluate unpleasant dream content participants completed a 14-item self-report questionnaire that assessed unpleasant dream experiences over the past month. Also, an eight-point scale ranging from 0 (never) to 7 (several time at night) used for participants to report on the overall frequency of recalled dreams.

Reported dream recall were classified as infrequent when reported at less than once a month, or frequent when reported at a frequency greater than once per week.

Statistical analysis was carried out with the SPSS version 19 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). After descriptive analyses, independent-sample t-test for quantitative variables andchi-square test for qualitative variables used for data analysis. P-values less than 0.05 were considered as a statistically significant level.

Results

From a total of 498 participants, 448 (mean age of 30.6 ± 10.9, male: 224) completed the questionnaires. Fifty were dropped from the analysis because of incomplete data. After assessment of BQ by two psychologists who were blind from the study, 72 subjects (16%) were diagnosed as definite cases of high risk for sleep apnea, and 376 (84%) were recognized as low risk for sleep apnea. So, we analyzed data regarding to the high and low risk for sleep apnea. Demographic characteristics are shown in Table 1. As shown as this table, risk factors of sleep apnea (male gender and high BMI, neck and waist circumference) were significantly higher in high risk patients than low risks.

Table 2 shows the responses obtained from both groups to the self-report questionnaire about unpleasant dream content. Results of 14-item self-report questionnaire showed that there were no significant differences on items related to the frequency of each unpleasant dream. Also, there were no significant differences on any of the general measures of dream recall. One hundred twenty subjects (32%) in low risk group reported less than once a month dream recall (infrequent) and ninety five subjects (25%) reported greater than once per week (frequent), whereas in high risk group, 22 subjects (30%) reported infrequent dream recall and 22 subjects (30%) reported frequent dream recall (p=0.48).

Discussion

In this study we investigated the relationship between risk of sleep apnea and unpleasant dream content in a large sample population. At the time of dream reporting, neither researcher nor subjects knew the medical sleep diagnosis for risk for sleep apnea. This is the most advantage of our study because, recent medical diagnosis for patient can affect mood or dream content.

There are many studies on the effect of external stimulants on dream content [3]. However; there are infrequent investigations on the effect of internal stimulants. Taking this idea that internal stimulants can affect dream content, it seems that sleep apnea which can affect many physiological processes, can also affects dream content. But, studies that carried out on the effects of sleep apnea on dream content are rare and yielded mixed results. Our finding shows that frequency of unpleasant dream in subjects who had high risk for sleep apnea were similar to normal control subjects. Despite a number of patients with high risk for sleep apnea reported unpleasant dreams, but a general effect of sleep apnea on unpleasant dream content has not been observed.

One mechanism to justification of this finding is that cognitive dysfunctions and attention deficit in people with sleep-related breathing disorders might affect reported dream [15,16]. It is demonstrated that there are strong association between dream length and verbal short-term memory in healthy elderly persons [3]. It seems that memory dysfunction might affect the processing of dream recall upon awakening. One other believable mechanism is that the mind adapts to the slow progress of apnea severity over the years. Surprisingly, it is suggested that it is necessary to conduct a study where participants wear a device which allows blocking the breathing completely to assess the effect of new apnea on dream content [3].

In a study by Schredl et al., correlation between respiratory disturbance indices (RDI) with different dream characteristics was investigated. They found a strong association between high RDI with more realistic and less bizarre dreams. The authors concluded that micro-arousals terminating breathing pauses might interfere with the dreaming process [11].

Fisher et al in a study on forty-seven patients with sleepiness and snoring which based on AHI grouped as AHI < 5, AHI 5 -14.9, and AHI = 15, found an increase in dream unpleasantness with increasing AHI. The AHI = 15 group had significantly more negative emotionally dreams than the AHI < 5 group (12). Also, previously, Carrasco et al, reported negative dream in sleep apnea patients (10). However, these studies appear to opposite to the findings of Pagel and Kwiatkowski that showed a decrease in percentage of patients with frequent recall of nightmares across their four AHI groups: 71.4% of those with AHI < 5 had frequent nightmares compared to 20.6% of those with AHI = 30. They concluded that depressed nightmare recall may due to the REMS suppression in OSA patients [6].

Another explanation of this variety in dream emotional tone among patients with sleep apnea could be that the sleep of these patients is so fragmented that it interferes with the dream processing and can barricade dream emotion to develop. This may be due to the high sleep apnea severity can disturbed sleep architecture [17,18].

Additionally, Merritt et al reported appearance of positive emotions in the first quartile and appearance of negative emotions in the last two quartiles of the dream. They suggested that dream emotions tend to go “from bad to worse.” [19]. It is possible that fragmented sleep in patients with sleep apnea is insufficient to consolidate sleep for dreams to go from bad to worse. So, dreams remain neutrally toned.

Regarding dream recall frequency in patients with sleep apnea, there are controversies about studies using a dream frequency scale. Studies showed that dream recall frequency in patients with sleep apnea, may be lower, higher or equal compared to that of control healthy subjects. Whereas Schredl et al. did not find any differences regarding to dream recall frequency in sleep apnea patients and control [20], in another study, this author reported that dream recall frequency in 323 patients with sleep apnea was lower than healthy controls [21]. In the third study [11] reported an increased dream recall frequency in forty-four sleep apnea patients.

Studies suggested two conflicted mechanisms which might affect dream recall frequency. First, light sleep and high arousal frequency might increase dream recall and, on the other hand, impaired cognitive functions which is common in patients with sleep apnea might impair dream recall [10,15,16].

We found no difference in reported unpleasant dreams between high and low risk subjects for sleep apnea. However, given the relatively low frequency of high risk subjects in our population (16%), the possibility of a type II error must be considered. Because of this low frequency, the application of the statistical analyses may be limited. Studies involving larger numbers of high risk patients are necessary to further clarify the relationship between unpleasant dreams and risk for sleep apnea. But our results are noteworthy, unless they are disproved.

In conclusion, this study shows that there is no association between frequency of unpleasant dream, dream recall and risk of sleep apnea. Light sleep, high arousal frequency and impaired short term memory might affect reported dream content as well as dream recall frequency.

References

Select your language of interest to view the total content in your interested language
Post your comment

Share This Article

Relevant Topics

Recommended Conferences

Article Usage

  • Total views: 12087
  • [From(publication date):
    September-2014 - Jun 22, 2018]
  • Breakdown by view type
  • HTML page views : 8311
  • PDF downloads : 3776
 

Post your comment

captcha   Reload  Can't read the image? click here to refresh

Peer Reviewed Journals
 
Make the best use of Scientific Research and information from our 700 + peer reviewed, Open Access Journals
International Conferences 2018-19
 
Meet Inspiring Speakers and Experts at our 3000+ Global Annual Meetings

Contact Us

Agri & Aquaculture Journals

Dr. Krish

[email protected]

+1-702-714-7001Extn: 9040

Biochemistry Journals

Datta A

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9037

Business & Management Journals

Ronald

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9042

Chemistry Journals

Gabriel Shaw

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9040

Clinical Journals

Datta A

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9037

Engineering Journals

James Franklin

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9042

Food & Nutrition Journals

Katie Wilson

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9042

General Science

Andrea Jason

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9043

Genetics & Molecular Biology Journals

Anna Melissa

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9006

Immunology & Microbiology Journals

David Gorantl

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9014

Materials Science Journals

Rachle Green

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9039

Nursing & Health Care Journals

Stephanie Skinner

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9039

Medical Journals

Nimmi Anna

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9038

Neuroscience & Psychology Journals

Nathan T

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9041

Pharmaceutical Sciences Journals

Ann Jose

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9007

Social & Political Science Journals

Steve Harry

[email protected]

1-702-714-7001Extn: 9042

 
© 2008- 2018 OMICS International - Open Access Publisher. Best viewed in Mozilla Firefox | Google Chrome | Above IE 7.0 version
Leave Your Message 24x7