Dersleri yüzünden oldukça stresli bir ruh haline sikiş hikayeleri bürünüp özel matematik dersinden önce rahatlayabilmek için amatör pornolar kendisini yatak odasına kapatan genç adam telefonundan porno resimleri açtığı porno filmini keyifle seyir ederek yatağını mobil porno okşar ruh dinlendirici olduğunu iddia ettikleri özel sex resim bir masaj salonunda çalışan genç masör hem sağlık hem de huzur sikiş için gelip masaj yaptıracak olan kadını gördüğünde porn nutku tutulur tüm gün boyu seksi lezbiyenleri sikiş dikizleyerek onları en savunmasız anlarında fotoğraflayan azılı erkek lavaboya geçerek fotoğraflara bakıp koca yarağını keyifle okşamaya başlar
Reach Us +44 3308186230

GET THE APP

Neonatal and Pediatric Medicine - Soccer Trained 6-11 year Old Children Demonstrate Better Executive Function Compared to Untrained Peers
ISSN: 2572-4983

Neonatal and Pediatric Medicine
Open Access

Our Group organises 3000+ Global Conferenceseries Events every year across USA, Europe & Asia with support from 1000 more scientific Societies and Publishes 700+ Open Access Journals which contains over 50000 eminent personalities, reputed scientists as editorial board members.

Open Access Journals gaining more Readers and Citations
700 Journals and 15,000,000 Readers Each Journal is getting 25,000+ Readers

This Readership is 10 times more when compared to other Subscription Journals (Source: Google Analytics)
  • Research Article   
  • Neonat Pediatr Med 2019, Vol 5(1): 176
  • DOI: 10.4172/2572-4983.1000176

Soccer Trained 6-11 year Old Children Demonstrate Better Executive Function Compared to Untrained Peers

Vaida Borkertiene*, Birute Zachariene and Arvydas Stasiulis
Department of Applied Physiology and Rehabilitation, Lithuanian Sports University, Kaunas, Lithuania
*Corresponding Author: Vaida Borkertiene, Department of Applied Physiology and Rehabilitation, Lithuanian Sports University, Kaunas, Lithuania, Tel: +37061110072, Email: vaida.sidlauskaite@gmail.com

Received: 09-Dec-2018 / Accepted Date: 17-Jan-2019 / Published Date: 26-Jan-2019 DOI: 10.4172/2572-4983.1000176

Abstract

The aim of the study was to compare the executive function (EF) between soccer trained (ST) and untrained (UT) 6-11 year old children. In addition, the relationship between EF and aerobic fitness was evaluated. In all, 18 ST children and 12 UT children participated in this study. EF was evaluated using the ADAM4 battery. Pulmonary oxygen uptake (VO2) was measured during constant treadmill walking (6 km/h, 4% grade, 6 min) and during an increasing walking exercise (modified Balke test). The ST children demonstrated better visual tracking and attention (95.97 ± 1.74%/94.4 ± 1.59%), response inhibition (96.49 ± 1.35%/93.72 ± 3.16%), speed of processing and alternating attention with a motor speed component (97.18 ± 3.0%/93.72 ± 2.16%) compared to the UT children (p<0.05). The time constant of VO2 kinetics during the constant walking exercise was shorter and the maximal VO2 was higher in the ST children (16.88 ± 2.19 s; 57.2 ± 6.0 ml/kg/min) compared to the UT children (20.99 ± 1.46 s; 48.1 ± 7.4 ml/kg/min) (p<0,05). There was a significant negative correlation between the cognitive tests and time constant of the VO2 kinetics. We concluded that the ST 6-11 year old children demonstrated and better as well as higher aerobic fitness EF compared with their UT peers. There was no correlation between aerobic fitness and EF indicators in any group of subjects.

Keywords: Oxygen uptake; Children; Executive function; Physical activity

Introduction

In times when children are less active [1], the significance of studies on the positive impact of sport on physical health, mental health, and cognitive functioning, is essential. Cognitive function is a general term reflecting different processes, such as working memory, memory, attention, pattern recognition, executive function (EF), intelligence, concept formation and reasoning and academic achievement [2]. Physical inactivity may be associated with lower cognitive function among adults and the elderly [2,3]. The influence of physical activity and aerobic capacity on cognitive function during childhood has only recently gained attention. There are hypotheses stating that physical activity may influence physiological changes in the brain and that physical activity may develop cognitive skills especially EF [3]. EF is a term that comprises inhibitory control, working memory, and attention flexibility that regulate goal-directed action and adaptive responses to novel, complex, or ambiguous situations [4]. These functions may be important prerequisite for successful learning in prepubertal children [5]. Within the preadolescent age range, metaanalyses have shown that enhanced cognitive functioning as a result of physical activity is most clearly seen in EF and attention [6] and the significant moderate positive effects of physical activity on children’s EF are usually observed [7]. The longitudinal physical activity programs have positive small to moderate effect on EF [8]. Recently Bidzan-Bluma and Lipowska [9] have concluded that engaging in sports in prepubertal age positively influences cognitive and emotional functions. However the results regarding the influence of sports on cognitive functions in prepubertal children are contradictory. Some authors report that sports have a positive influence [10,11] particularly on EF. Others, however, do not confirm this positive influence of physical activity on cognitive functioning [12,13].

Hillman and colleagues have reported more improvement from pretest to post-test in some aspects of cognitive performance and measures of brain function in children aged 8 to 9 years who followed the FITKids intervention versus a control group [14]. Schmidt et al. have found that in 10- to 12-year-old children the group that participated in a cognitively- and physically-demanding exercise intervention (team games) showed more improvement from pre-test to post-test on cognitive flexibility (i.e, being able to shift from one task to another) than the group receiving an aerobic intervention with low cognitive demands or the group receiving standard physical education with low physical and low cognitive demands [15]. On the other hand cross-sectional studies have demonstrated that aerobic fitness is positively related to executive control, with more fit children exhibiting superior attention, decision-making ability, and differential brain function compared with their lesser-fit peers [16,17].

There is a paucity of publications that investigate the impact of sports on prepurbetal children cognitive functions, or explore which cognitive functions are developed by which sporting disciplines. Such knowledge would be useful in developing training programs for preadolescents, aimed at improving cognitive functions that may guide both researchers and practitioners relative to the wide range of benefits that result from physical activity.

Soccer training and playing is associated both with development of motor skilled, tactical thinking, and increase in aerobic and anaerobic fitness. So, we have hypothesized that several years of soccer training should improve EF in prepubertal children.

Cross-sectional studies [16,17] have demonstrated that aerobic fitness is positively related to executive control, with more fit children exhibiting superior attention, decision-making ability, and differential brain function compared with their less-fit peers.

The aim of the study was to compare the EF between ST and UT 6-10 year old children. In addition, the relationship between EF and aerobic fitness was evaluated.

Participants

In all, 12 untrained (UT) and 18 soccer trained (ST) 6-11 year old children participated in this study (Table 1). UT children we invited children from general schools. These children did not have any specific physical education, except for physical education lessons in school (2 times/week, 45 min/time), which are obligatory for every healthy children in Lithuania. The ST children were soccer players with a training experience of 5.0 ± 1.5 years. All ST group children were invited from Kaunas soccer club. Participants had soccer training 3 times/week, for two hours. Children participated in soccer events, regional championships and soccer matches. Written informed consent was obtained from the parents, and written assent was obtained from the participants. The criteria that eliminated the participants from research were: heart , diabetes, epilepsy, and musculoskeletal problems, first signs of puberty. Ethical approval for the research was obtained from the Kaunas Regional Ethics Committee.

  UT (n=12, 7 boys, 5 girls) ST (n=18, 13 boys, 5 girls)
Age (yr) 9.41 ± 1.92 9.95 ± 1.49
Height (m) 1.42 ± 0.18 1.44 ± 0.10
Weight (kg) 38.08 ± 7.1 37.96 ± 6.42
BMI (kg/m2 18.88 ± 0.94 18.55 ± 1.43
UT: Untrained Children; ST: Sport Trained children

Table 1: Subject characteristics.

Materials And Methods

Anthropometry

The heights of the participants were measured to the nearest 0,01 m using a stadiometer. Body mass was measured using a body composition analyzer, the TBF-300 (Japanese). BMI was calculated as the body mass (kg) divided by the height squared (m2).

Constant walking exercise (CWE)

The exercise was performed on a motor driven treadmill (HP Cosmos, USA). To familiarize the participants with treadmill walking and give them a slight warm-up, the exercise started with slow walking on treadmill (at 3 km/h, 0% grade) for 3 min. Then, the participants stood on the sides of treadmill for 1 min. During this period of time treadmill speed was increased to 6 km/h and the slope to 4%. At the end of the minute, the participants stepped on the moving treadmill and continued walking for 6 min. After this, the treadmill was stopped and the participants rested in the supine position for 5 min.

Increasing walking exercise (IWE)

The modified Balke test was performed on the same treadmill. Following one min of standing, the participants started walking at 5.6 km/h and 0% for three min. After this, they continued walking at the same speed while the treadmill grade was increased every min by 2%. The subjects were verbally encouraged to give maximal effort during the test and continued walking until voluntary fatigue (exhaustion).

Pulmonary gas exchange

The pulmonary gas exchange parameters were continuously measured breath-by-breath with a portable telemetric system (Oxycon Mobile, Jaeger, Germany). The flow-volume sensor and the gas analyzer (gas mixtures containing 5% CO2 and 16% O2 were used) were calibrated using automatic calibration procedures, as provided by Jaeger, before each test session

Heart rate

The HR was recorded with a “Polar” system. Children wore a pediatric wireless chest strap telemetry system to monitor the HR.

Cognitive function evaluation

The Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Metrics version 4 (ANAM4) was used to evaluate cognitive function. The four performance tests were, in the order of administration: the 2-Choice Reaction Time test (2CRT), Code Substitution-Learning (CSL), Go/No-Go test, and Simple Reaction Time (PRO) (Table 2). The specific tests assess areas or domains of cognitive functioning, including attention/concentration, reaction time, processing speed, working memory, visuospatial skills, motor speed, memory, reasoning and problem solving. Among a number of output measures, we utilized the percent correct #((NumCorr/(NumCorr+Numinc+NumLapse))) and throughput of correct responses #((=NumCorr/((NumCorr +Numinc)*Mean RT+NumLApse*Timeout))). The throughput metric is defined as the number of correct responses per minute and is thus a useful metric for overall cognitive efficiency. It took ~15-20 min for the participants to complete the tests [18].

Test Description of test Cognitive domain
2CRT This test measures choice reaction time by presenting the user with a “*” or “o” on the display. The subject is instructed to respond as quickly as possible by pressing the designated button for each stimulus as soon as the stimulus appears. This test measures choice reaction time.
CSL In this test, the subject must compare a displayed digit-symbol pair with a set of defined digit-symbol pairs or the key. The user presses designated buttons to indicate whether the pair in question represents a correct or incorrect mapping relative to the key. In the learning phase, the defined pairs are presented on the screen along with the digit-symbol pair in question. Results of this test are used as an index of complex scanning, visual tracking, and attention.
Go/No-Go The subject is presented with two characters, “x” and “o”. The subject is instructed to respond as quickly as possible to the “x” by pressing a button each time the stimulus appears. When the “o” appears, the user is to do nothing. This test assesses response inhibition.
PRO The subject clicks the left mouse button (single-button response) when an asterisk stimulus is presented on the screen. This stimulus is presented at different intervals for 40 trials, and the reaction time for each trial is recorded. This subtest assesses reaction time. Results of this test are used as an index of attention and visuo-motor response timing.

Table 2: Descriptions of the cognitive tests [18].

#NumCorr: Number of trials with a correct response.

Numinc: Number of trials with an incorrect response.

NumLapse: Number of trials in which no response was made in the allotted time.

Meant RT: Mean response time of all items (correct & incorrect).

Study design

The participants came to the laboratory 3 times. The participants were asked to do cognitive tests on the first arrival. First, the participants were learning to do cognitive tests (performed 2 trials), and then, the true test was recorded for evaluation. It took approximately 20-30 min to complete the cognitive tests battery. The participants were asked to not undergo intense exercise at least for 24 h before the second arrival to the laboratory. During this visit, two CWE sessions (separated by 45 min) were performed. After 3 to 7 days, the participants arrived at the lab for the third time and performed IWE.

Data analysis

VO2 kinetics during CWE was determined using a monoexponential model. The following equation was used to model mono exponential VO2 response kinetics: VO2(t) = VO2(b) + A(1_e_t/τ), where VO2(t) is the VO2 at any time point; VO2(b) is the baseline VO2 during 30 s before exercise; A is the amplitude of the VO2 response; and (1_e_t/τ) is the exponential function describing the rate at which the VO2 is rising toward the amplitude. In the exponential function, t is time; τ is the time constant. The initial 20 seconds of the test were not included in the analysis.3

The peak oxygen uptake (VO2peak) was determined as the highest VO2 within a 20-sec period during the IWE.

Statistical analysis

All values are expressed as the mean ± standard deviation. The comparisons of the results were analyzed by one-way ANOVA. Significance was accepted at p

Results

No statistically significant difference of age, height, weight or body mass index was found between ST and UT groups (Table 1).

The time constant of oxygen uptake kinetics during constant walking exercise was significantly different (p<0.05) between the UT (20.99 ± 1.46) and the ST (16.99 ± 2.37) 6-11 year old children.

The VO2 peak during increasing walking exercise was significantly different between the UT (48.1 ± 7.41) and ST (57.26 ± 5.24) 6-11 year old children. The VO2 peak was significantly different among the age groups in the ST and UT groups (p<0.05).

The executive function results are shown in Table 3.

ANAM4 Group ProcCOR p Troughtput p
CSL UT 94.4 ( ± 1.59) 0.013* 58.39 ( ± 7.07) 0.675
ST 95.97 ( ± 1.74) 58.00 ( ± 5.76)
Go/No/Go UT 93.72 ( ± 3.16) 0.006* -  
ST 96.49 ( ± 1.35)
2CRT UT 93.72 ( ± 2.16) 0.000* 136.04 ( ± 10.2) 0.697
ST 97.18 ( ± 3.00) 134.56 ( ± 8.57)
PRO UT 93.96 ( ± 1.72) 0.213 90.15 ( ± 1.64) 0.049*
ST 94.32 ( ± 1.94) 92.17 ( ± 2.74)
*p<0.05

Table 3: Results of executive function in untrained and soccer trained 6-11 years old children.

The CSL results were significantly different between the ST (95.97 ± 1.74%) and UT (94.4 ± 1.59%) children (p<0.05), and the Go/No-Go test results were significantly different between the ST (96.49 ± 1.35%) and UT (93.72 ± 3.16%) children (p<0.05). The 2CRT results were significantly different between the ST (97.18 ± 3.00%) and UT (93.72 ± 2.16%) 6-11 year old children (p<0.05).

Correlation in ST and UT groups between EF and aerobic fitness are shown in Table 4.

  Correlations
ST group UT group
CHR GNG CSL PRO CHR GNG CSL PRO
VO2 peak Pearson Correlation 0.084 0.082 0.063 -0.218 .613* -0.081 .684* .638*
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.742 0.745 0.805 0.386 0.034 0.802 0.014 0.026
N 18 18 18 18 12 12 12 12
t1 Pearson Correlation -0.155 -0.413 -0.411 0.225 -0.313 0.265 -0.166 -0.321
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.539 0.088 0.091 0.369 0.322 0.406 0.606 0.309
N 18 18 18 18 12 12 12 12
*Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)

Table 4: Correlation in ST and UT groups between EF and aerobic fitness.

There was no significant correlation between aerobic fitness and EF indicators in any group of subjects.

Discussion

The main finding of this study is that 6-11 year old ST children demonstrated better a EF compared to their untrained peers. In addition, the aerobic fitness was significantly higher in ST group as well. But there was no significant correlation between aerobic fitness and EF indicators in any group of subjects.

Our results suggest that both training and playing soccer for several years and/or an increased aerobic capacity may have an influence on better EF. Several metaanalysis have revealed that physical activity has small to moderate positive effect on EF in prepubertal children [6-9].

However, the studies of the benefits of sports for EF are scarse. It has be suggested that sports might benefit EF more than aerobic exercise alone, since besides improving fitness, sports challenge EF (requiring sustained attention, working memory, and disciplined action) and bring joy, pride, and social bonding [19]. Children getting traditional Tae-Kwon-Do training were found to show greater gains of EF than children in standard physical education [20]. It has been recently shown that basketball activities enhanced EF and working memory scanning speed in adolescents [21]. Hillman and colleagues have reported more improvement from pre-test to post-test in some aspects of cognitive performance and measures of brain function in children aged 8 to 9 years who followed the FITKids intervention versus a control group [14]. Schmidt et al. have found that in 10- to 12-year-old children the group that participated in a cognitively- and physically-demanding exercise intervention (team games) showed more improvement from pre-test to post-test on cognitive flexibility (i.e., being able to shift from one task to another) than the group receiving an aerobic intervention with low cognitive demands or the group receiving standard physical education with low physical and low cognitive demands [15]. So, it seems that soccer playing and training may be very effective for EF in prepubertal children. Our findings show that, 6-11 year ST children demonstrate a higher processing speed and alternating attention with a motor speed component, reaction time, attention, response inhibition and an index of attention compared with untrained children.

Several mechanisms might explain the effects of physical activity on cognitive functions. First, acute physical activity is thought to immediately elevate the child’s level of physiological arousal, which in turn facilitates the cognitive performance by an increased allocation of attention [22]. In addition acute physical activity leads to an increase of neurotransmitters (e.g. epinephrine, dopamine, brain-derived neurotrophic factors), which may enhance cognitive processes [23].

Secondly, regular aerobic physical activity over several weeks is thought to improve aerobic fitness and consequently improve cognitive performance [24]. It is suggested that EF is more strongly related to physical fitness than it is to physical activity [25]. Physical activity enhances the angiogenesis and neurogenesis in areas of the brain that support memory and learning, subsequently enhancing cognitive performance [16,26]. More recently, other researchers argue cognitively engaging physical activity is more beneficial for cognition. Physical activities with a relatively high cognitive engagement (e.g. sports games are suggested to have more effect on EF compared to physical activities with a relatively low cognitive engagement (e.g. long distance running, which involves more automated movements) [15,27]. Sport trained children are characterized as having larger brain volumes in the basal ganglia and hippocampus. This relates to a higher performance on tasks of memory and cognitive control and also to a higher brain function during tasks of cognitive control and better scores on tests of academic achievement [28]. Ortega et al., [29] proved that all physical fitness components, cardiorespiratory fitness, muscular strength and speed agility, were significantly related to the shapes of the subcortical brain nuclei. These associations were positive, indicating that a higher level of fitness in childhood is related to both expansions and contractions in certain regions of the accumbency, amygdala, caudate, hippocampus, pallidum, putamen and thalamus. Physical activity has a positive benefit for brain structure, brain function, cognition, and school achievement among 7-10 year old children [28]. Physical activity and healthy diets have a positive influence on cognitive outcomes even in early childhood [30]. Additionally, single bouts of moderate intensity exercise may be an effective means for modulating neural activity and can help regulate attention [31].

The sport trained children in our study demonstrated a 20% higher VO2 peak than their untrained peers. This is a larger difference than that usually found in exercise training studies, in which the increase due to training or physical activity is approximately 5-6% [32,33]. In reviewing 15 studies, including 22 experimental groups of pre-pubertal (younger than 11 years old) child athletes, Armstrong and Barker [34] found a mean VO2 max improvement of only~6.5%. Nevertheless, pediatric studies employing high-intensity training have typically shown VO2 max gains of well less than 9% [35-37]. The participants in our study were training for soccer for several years which involves both long aerobic and intensive aerobic explosive strength activities. This may explain the rather high difference in aerobic capacity between the groups. It is even better than with recently published data of the study in which the group performing the high-intensity exercises increased their cardiorespiratory fitness by approximately 10% [38]. The physiological mechanisms of such a difference are not quite clear yet (for a detailed analysis [39]). In short, all of the training induced VO2 max changes must be related to an increased cardiac output, an arterio-venous O2 difference, or both. There are data suggesting that the maximal VO2 differences of youth athletes and non-athletes are similar. It is suggested that pre-pubertal children are not able to activate fast motor units during daily activities or even during training, so they have less potential to increase the VO2 peak and other aerobic parameters during training [40]. Therefore, training induced changes in the VO2 peak have been associated with increases in oxygen delivery. Both morphological and functional adaptations of the myocardium have been hypothesized as explanations for an augmented stroke volume following training. Increases in the blood volume, left ventricular dimensions and mass, intraventricular and posterior wall thickness, shortening fraction, and ejection fraction have been postulated, but empirical evidence is sparse and conflicting [34,41].

In our study, a faster VO2 time constant was found in the ST 6-11 year old children compared with their untrained peers. The difference (19.5%) is similar to that in the case of the VO2 peak. Data on youth VO2 kinetics trainability are, however, sparse and largely reliant on four comparative studies of trained soccer players and swimmers compared to untrained youth from two research groups [41]. As the trained pubertal girls exhibited faster [HHb] kinetics and HR kinetics than their untrained pubertal girls, the authors suggested that the faster τ could be attributed to both enhanced oxygen delivery and enhanced oxygen utilization [33,41].

McNarry and colleagues [42,43] contrasted the responses of swim-trained and untrained pre-pubertal and pubertal girls at the onset of heavy intensity exercise and indicated a shorter phase II τ in both trained groups than in their untrained peers during an arm-cranking exercise and a shorter Phase II τ during a leg-cycling exercise in the trained pubertal girls. On the basis of observing faster HR kinetics and HHb kinetics in the trained pubertal girls, McNarry et al. [42] hypothesized that their shorter phase II τ was a function of both enhanced oxygen delivery to the muscles and enhanced oxygen utilization in the muscles.

Some authors found that aerobic fitness is positively related to executive control, with more fit children exhibiting superior attention, decision-making ability, and differential brain function compared with their lesser-fit peers [16,17]. In our study the corerlation between EF and aerobic fitness within groups was not significant. It may be associated with small number of participants in groups and with not large dispersion of data correlated. On the other hand correlation does not show the causality of relationship so we cannot exlude the role of higher aerobic fitness in better EF in ST group due to mechanisms discussed above.

The limitation of the study may be associated with cross sectional study design which does not allow to assess the influence of initial fitness and cognitive function level on the differences found between groups. In the future there is need for longitudinal study of children involved in regular sports training.

Conclusion

6-11 year old ST children demonstrated better a EF compared to their untrained peers. The aerobic fitness was significantly higher in ST group as well. There was no significant correlation between aerobic fitness and EF indicators in any group of subjects.

References

  1. van Stralen MM, Yıldırım M, Wulp A, te Velde SJ, Verloigne M, et al. (2013) Measured sedentary time and physical activity during the school day of European 10- to 12-year-old children: the ENERGY project. J Sci Med Sport 17: 201-206.
  2. Tomporowski PD, Lambourne K, Okumura MS (2011) Physical activity interventions and children's mental function: An introduction and overview. Prev Med 52: S3-S9.
  3. Lonx X, Benischek A, Dewey D, Lebel C (2017) Age-related functional brain changes in young children. Neuroimage 155: 322-330.
  4. Hughes C, Graham A, Grayson A (2005) Executive function in childhood: Development and disorder. In: Oates JM, Grayson A (Eds.) Cognitive and language development in children. Open University Press, Oxford, USA. pp205-230.
  5. Chang YK, Labban JD, Gapin JI, Etnier JL (2012) The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Brain Res 1453: 87-101.
  6. Verburgh L, Königs M, Scherder EJ, Oosterlaan J (2014) Physical exercise and executive functions in preadolescent children, adolescents and young adults: a meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med 48: 973-979.
  7. de Greeffa JW, Boskerb RJ, Oosterlaand J, Visschera C, Hartmana E (2018) Effects of physical activity on executive functions, attention and academic performance in preadolescent children: a meta-analysis. J Sci Med Sport 21: 501-507.
  8. Bidzan-Bluma I, Lipowska M (2018) Physical Activity and Cognitive Functioning of Children: A Systematic Review. Int J Environ Res Public Health 15: 800.
  9. Hinkle JS, Tuckman BW, Sampson JP (1993) The psychology, physiology, and creativity of middle schoolaerobic exercisers. Elem Sch Guidance Couns 28: 133-145.
  10. Carson V, Hunter S, Kuzik N, Wiebe SA, Spence JC (2016) Systematic review of physical activity and cognitive development in early childhood. J Sci Med Sport 19: 573-578.
  11. Zervas Y, Danis A, Klissouras V (1991) Influence of physical exertion on mental performance with reference to training. Percept Mot Skills 72: 1215-1221.
  12. Ahamed Y, Macdonald H, Reed K, Naylor PJ, Liu-Ambrose T (2007) School-Based Physical Activity Does Not Compromise Children’s Academic Performance. Med Sci Sports Exerc 39: 371-376.
  13. Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Castelli DM, Khan NA, Raine LB, et al. (2014) Effects of the FITKids randomized controlled trial on executive control and brain function. Pediatrics 134: e1063-1071.
  14. Schmidt M, Jäger K, Egger F, Roebers CM, Conzelmann A (2015) Cognitively engaging chronic physical activity, but not aerobic exercise, affects executive functions in primary school children: a group randomized controlled trial. J Sport Exerc Psychol 37: 575-591.
  15. Hillman CH, Buck SM, Themanson JR, Pontifex MB, Castelli DM (2009) Aerobic fitness and cognitive develop-ment: event-related brain potential and task performance indices of executive control in preadolescent children. Dev Psychol 45: 114-129.
  16. Pontifex MB, Raine LB, Johnson CR, Chaddock L, Voss MW (2011) Cardiorespiratory fitness and the flexible modulation of cognitive control in preadolescent children. J Cogn Neurosci 23: 1332-1345.
  17. Ibarra S (2008) Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Metrics. Center for the Study of Human Operator Performance, USA.
  18. Diamond A, Lee K (2011) Interventions shown to Aid Executive Function Development in Children 4-12 Years Old. Science 19: 959-964.
  19. Lakes KD, Hoyt WT (2004) Promoting self-regulation through school-based martial arts training. Applied Developmental Psychology 25: 283-302.
  20. Cooper SB, Dring KJ, Morris JG, Sunderland C, Bandelow S (2018) High intensity intermittent games-based activity and adolescents' cognition: moderating effect of physical fitness. BMC Public Health 18: 603.
  21. Tomporowski PD (2003) Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychol 112: 297-324.
  22. Roig M, Nordbrandt S, Geertsen SS, Nielsen JB (2013) The effects of cardiovascular exercise on human memory: a review with meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 37: 1645-1666.
  23. Etnier JL, Salazar W, Landers DM, Petruzzello SJ, Han M, et al. (1997) The influence of physical fitness and exercise upon cognitive functioning: a meta-analysis. J Sport Exerc Psychol 19: 249-277.
  24. Ishihara T, Sugasawa S, Matsuda Y, Mizuno M (2017) Relationship between sports experience and executive function in 6-12-year-old children: independence from physical fitness and moderation by gender. Dev Sci 21: e12555.
  25. Isaacs KR, Anderson BJ, Alcantara AA, Black JE, Greenough WT (1992) Exercise and the brain: angiogenesis inthe adult rat cerebellum after vigorous physical activity and motor skill learning. J Celeb Blood Flow Metab 12: 110-119.
  26. Vazou S, Pesce C, Lakes K, Smiley-Oyen A (2016) More than one road leads to Rome: a narrativereview and meta-analysis of physical activity intervention effects on cognition in youth. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol 2016: 1-26.
  27. Chaddock-Heyman L, Hillman CH, Cohen NJ, Kramer AF (2014) The importance of physical activity and aerobic fitness for cognitive control and memory in children. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 79: 25-50.
  28. Ortega FB, Campos D, Cadenas-Sanchez C, Altmae S, Martínez-Zaldívar C, et al. (2017) Physical fitness and shapes of subcortical brain structures in children. Br J Nutr 27: 1-10.
  29. Tandon PS, Tovar A, Jayasuriya AT, Welker E, Schober DJ, et al. (2016) The relationship between physical activity and diet and young childrens’s cognitive development: A systematic review. Preventive Medicine Reports 3: 379-390.
  30. Drollette ES, Scudder MR, Raine LB, Moore RD, Saliba BJ, et al. (2014) Acute exercise facilitates brain function and cognition in children who need it most: An ERP study of individual differences in inhibitory control capacity. Dev Cogn Neurosci 7: 53-64.
  31. Dencker M, Bugge A, Hermansen B, Andersen LB (2009) Objectively measured daily physical activity related to aerobic fitness in young children. J Sports Sci 28: 139-145.
  32. McNarry MA, Farr C, Middlebrooke A, Welford D, Breese B, et al. (2014) Aerobic Function and Muscle Deoxygenation Dynamics during Ramp Exercise in Children. Med Sci Sports Exerc 15: 1877-1884.
  33. Armstrong N, Barker AR (2009) Oxygen uptake kinetics in children and adolescents: a review. Pediatr Exerc Sci 21: 130-147.
  34. Mahon AD, Vaccaro P (1989) Ventilatory threshold and VO2max changes in children following endurance training. Med Sci Sports Exerc 21: 425-431.
  35. Mandigout S, Lecoq AM, Courteix D, Guenon P, Obert P (2001) Effect of gender in response to an aerobic training programme in prepubertal children. Acta Paediatr90: 9-15.
  36. Rotstein A, Dotan R, Bar-Or O, Tenenbaum G (1986) Effect of training on anaerobic threshold, maximal aerobic power and anaerobic performance of preadolescent boys. Int J Sports Med 7: 281-286.
  37. Reyes-Amigo T, Gómez M, Gallardo M, Palmeira A (2017) Effectiveness of high-intensity interval training on cardiorespiratory fitness and body composition in preadolescents: a systematic review. European Journal of Human Movement 39: 32-47.
  38. Armstrong N (2017) Pediatric Aerobic Fitness and Trainability. Pediatr Exerc Sci 29: 8-13.
  39. Dotan R. Children's Aerobic Trainability and Related Questions. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2017; 88(4):377-383.
  40. Amstrong N, McNarry M (2016) Aerobic Fitness and Trainability in Healthy Youth: Gaps in Our Knowledge. Pediatr Exerc Sci 28: 171-177.
  41. McNarry MA, Welsman JR, Jones AM (2010) Influence of training and maturity status on the cardiopulmonary responses to ramp incremental cycle and upper body exercise in girls. J Appl Physiol 110: 375-381.
  42. Winlove MA, Jones AM, Welsman JR (2010) Influence of training status and exercise modality on pulmonary O(2) uptake kinetics in pre-pubertal girls. Eur J Appl Physiol 108: 1169-1179.

Citation: Borkertiene V, Zachariene B, Stasiulis A (2019) Soccer Trained 6-11 year Old Children Demonstrate Better Executive Function Compared to Untrained Peers. Neonat Pediatr Med 5: 176. DOI: 10.4172/2572-4983.1000176

Copyright: © 2019 Borkertiene V, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Top