ISSN: 2155-6105
Journal of Addiction Research & Therapy
Make the best use of Scientific Research and information from our 700+ peer reviewed, Open Access Journals that operates with the help of 50,000+ Editorial Board Members and esteemed reviewers and 1000+ Scientific associations in Medical, Clinical, Pharmaceutical, Engineering, Technology and Management Fields.

Smoking Susceptibility and its Predictors among Adolescents in China: Evidence from Ningbo City

Cheng Huang1*, Jeffrey P Koplan2, Jing Liu3, Changwei Li4, Jessica Silvaggio5, Amanda K MacGurn6, Tao Zhang7, Michael P Erikson8 and Pam Redmon2

1Department of Global Health, George Washington University, USA

2Global Health Institute, Emory University, USA

3School of Criminal Justice, State University of New York at Albany, USA

4Department of Epidemiology, Tulane University, USA

5Department of Epidemiology, Emory University, USA

6Hubert Department of Global Health, Emory University, USA

7Ningbo Center for Disease Control and Prevention, China

8School of Public Health, Georgia State University, USA

*Corresponding Author:
Cheng Huang
Department of Global Health
George Washington University, USA
E-mail: chenghuang@gwu.edu

Received June 06, 2012; Accepted September 04, 2012; Published September 07, 2012

Citation: Huang C, Koplan JP, Liu J, Li C, Silvaggio J, et al. (2012) Smoking Susceptibility and its Predictors among Adolescents in China: Evidence from Ningbo City. J Addict Res Ther S8:004. doi:10.4172/2155-6105.S8-004

Copyright: © 2012 Huang C. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Visit for more related articles at Journal of Addiction Research & Therapy

Abstract

Susceptibility to smoking is a risk factor of actual adolescent smoking behaviors. This study aimed to estimate the rate of smoking susceptibility and its predictors in China with a sample of 4,695 junior high school students in Ningbo, China. Core questions from the Global Youth Tobacco Survey (GYTS) were adapted to the China context and administered to these students. The rate of smoking susceptibility, measured by “Do you foresee yourself taking up smoking in the next 12 months”, is 6.1%. Results from logistic regression suggested that among boys, adolescents’ health knowledge that smoking can cause lung cancer (OR=2.73), the belief that smoking can help people relax (OR=2.32), and self-report of never having seen anti-smoking information on campus (OR=1.80) predicted increased susceptibility to smoking. Conversely, the belief that boys who smoke are less attractive (OR=0.64), that parents will have a problem with their child smoking (OR=0.50), having no friends or classmates who smoke (OR=0.22), and not seeing teachers smoke in the previous week (OR=0.61) predicted decreased susceptibility to smoking. Findings for girls were similar. This study suggested the need for comprehensive programs aiming to improve family, peer, and school environments to decrease smoking susceptibility among adolescents.

Keywords

Smoking susceptibility; Adolescent; Peer effect; Social learning; China

Introduction

Smoking has become an emerging public health problem among youth, with initiation occurring at younger ages [1,2]. The Global Youth Tobacco Survey (GYTS), a school-based survey of students aged 13-15 years old in 131 countries suggested that 8.9% of students were current smokers, and one in five never smokers reported that they were susceptible to smoking in the next year [3]. Ample evidence suggests that adolescents are usually susceptible to smoking when they do not show a firm commitment to not smoking [4], and that there is a strong correlation between smoking susceptibility and actual adolescent smoking behavior [4-7].

Existing studies have identified a series of psychosocial factors relating to smoking susceptibility among adolescents [8-10]. For example, susceptibility to smoking is positively associated with smoking behaviors of friends and family members [9,10]. School environment also matters [8,9]. Exposure to and perceived usefulness of school prevention programs are negatively associated with susceptibility to smoking. These effects are not gender blind. For example, for girls only, billboard tobacco advertising increases the risk of susceptibility and classroom prevention decreases risk; but for boys only, attendance at schools with higher prevalence of tobacco use increases the risk of susceptibility and anti-smoking media messages decrease the risk [8]. Most of these studies were conducted in the US or Canada, and very little evidence came from low-income countries [8].

The purpose of the current study was two-fold: first, we examined the influence of knowledge, attitudes around smoking behavior, peers, family, and school environments on smoking susceptibility among adolescents in China- the biggest producer and consumer of tobacco worldwide. Second, we tested whether these effects are gender-specific.

Materials and Methods

Data

An eastern coastal city in China with a total population of 2.21 million, Ningbo was one of the 17 city grantees of the Emory Global Health Institute-China Tobacco Control Partnership Program. This initiative was launched in 2009 with support from the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation, aiming to build sustainable, comprehensive, citylevel tobacco control programs to prevent smoking initiation among youth, young adults, and women, to promote cessation among adults and youth, and to eliminate exposure to environmental tobacco smoke. Ningbo primarily targeted students in elementary schools and junior high schools. At the onset of the project, a series of surveys were conducted by Ningbo Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) to obtain tobacco-related benchmark information from school students. The survey of junior high school students was carried out in Ningbo’s six districts, including Haishu, Jiangdong, Jiangbei, Beilun, Zhenhai, and Yinzhou. Eight were chosen out of 223 junior high schools using stratified cluster sampling, with four in urban areas, two in suburban areas, and two in rural areas. All students from grades seven to eight in these eight schools were included in the survey [11]. The final sample consisted of 4,695 students- 2,440 boys and 2,255 girls- completing the survey. The students’ ages ranged from 10 to 18 years old, with 97.8% of the students were 12-14 years old.

The survey questions were adapted from the core questions of the Global Youth Tobacco Survey (GYTS), including introduction, tobacco-related knowledge, attitudes and behaviors, policies and regulations, and smoking status. These questions were adapted to China’s context, translated into Chinese, and pilot-tested. The survey was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Ningbo Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). Enumerators from the Ningbo CDC received training prior to survey administration and adhered to strict procedures. The survey was anonymous and informed consent was obtained from participants. Teachers were not present when the students were taking the survey, and each returned questionnaire was checked for completeness by the enumerators.

Measures

Smoking susceptibility: We measured susceptibility to smoking, the outcome variable of this study, with a single question: “Do you foresee yourself taking up smoking in the next 12 months?” Those responding positively were coded as susceptible to smoking. Potential risk factors for smoking susceptibility were also measured, including smoking-related knowledge, attitudes towards smoking, peer dynamics, family characteristics, and school environment.

Knowledge: Specifically, we measured students’ knowledge of harm caused by smoking by asking “Do you think smoking can cause the following diseases: bronchitis, hypertension, lung cancer, heart attack, and stroke?” Responses for each type of disease were given a binary code of “no” vs. “yes”.

Attitudes and social norms: We measured attitudes towards smoking and perceived social norms regarding smoking with three questions: “Do you think smoking can help people relax?”, “Do you think boys who smoke are more attractive?”, and “Do you think girls who smoke are more attractive?”.

Peer dynamics and family characteristics: We measured peer dynamics with the question: “Based on your knowledge, do any of your friends or classmates smoke?” For family characteristics, respondents were asked, “Do you think your parents will not mind you smoking after you grow up” and “In a typical week, how many days do people smoke in your presence at home?”

School environment: School environment was measured with questions including the following: “In the past week, did you see your teachers smoking?”, “During the past semester, did your school distribute any tobacco control materials to you?”, “In the past semester, did you watch any videos or TV programs on tobacco control on campus?”, “In the past semester, did anyone at your school teach you that smokers would eventually have yellow teeth, wrinkles, and bad smell”, “In the past semester, did anyone at your school teach you skills on how to refuse smoking”, and “Do you often see anti-smoking information on campus?”

Analytic strategy

Logistic regression analysis was used to estimate the crude associations between smoking acceptability and each of the five types of risk factors, including knowledge, attitudes, peer influences, family influences, and school environment in separate models (Model 1-5), controlling for grade and location of schools. We also assessed the independent effect of each cluster of risk factors when other clusters were included (Model 6). Because gender differences have been observed in the effects of predictive factors, separate models were estimated for boys and girls. We also adjusted standard errors to accommodate clustering effects at the class level, because students from the same class presumably shared similar experiences in the school environment. All analyses were conducted using SAS 9.0, and we reported odds ratios (ORs) with their 95% confidence intervals (CI).

Results

Table 1 presents characteristics of students by susceptibility to smoking in our sample. Of the 4,695 students, 6.1% were susceptible to smoking. That is, they lacked a firm commitment to not smoking in the following 12 months. The characteristics of students who were susceptible to smoking were significantly different from those who were not susceptible, except for the health knowledge that smoking can cause hypertension, heart attack, and stroke. For example, higher percentages of susceptible students held pro-smoking beliefs, such as the belief that smoking can help people relax (36.1% vs. 11.4%), and that boys who smoke (14.8% vs. 3.4%) and girls who smoke (19.3% vs. 4.6%) are more attractive.

Characteristics No (n=4,410) Yes (n=285) P-value
Area of residence
Urban 63.9% 56.5% 0.0245
Suburban 24.0% 27.0%  
Rural 12.1% 16.5%  
Grade      
7th 49.9% 35.1% <0.0001
8th 50.1% 64.9%  
Do you think smoking can cause bronchitis?
No 13.3% 23.5% <0.0001
Yes 86.8% 76.5%  
Do you think smoking can cause hypertension?
No 63.1% 62.8% 0.912
Yes 36.9% 37.2%  
Do you think smoking can cause lung cancer?
No 3.6% 13.0% <0.0001
Yes 96.4% 87.0%  
Do you think smoking can cause heart attack?
No 63.6% 66.0% 0.4179
Yes 36.4% 34.0%  
Do you think smoking can cause stroke?
No 69.3% 68.8% 0.8504
Yes 30.7% 31.2%  
Do you think smoking can help people relax?
Yes 11.4% 36.1% <0.0001
No 88.6% 63.9%  
Do you think smoking boys are more attractive?
More 3.4% 14.8% <0.0001
Less 63.9% 37.0%  
No difference 32.7% 48.2%  
Do you think smoking girls are more attractive?
More 4.6% 19.3% <0.0001
Less 72.1% 51.2%  
No difference 23.3% 29.5%  
As far as you know, does any of your friends or classmates smoke?
None 80.7% 47.7% <0.0001
Some 18.0% 43.2%  
Most or all 1.2% 9.1%  
Do you think your parents would not mind your smoking after you grow up?
No 7.4% 14.7% <0.0001
Yes 92.7% 85.3%  
In a typical week, how many days do people smoke in your presence at home
None 40.0% 29.8% 0.0007
1-7 days 60.0% 70.2%  
In the past week, did you see your teachers smoking?
No 66.7% 47.4% <0.0001
Yes 33.3% 52.6%  
In the past semester, did the school distribute any tobacco control material to you?
No or don't know 54.6% 57.9% 0.2771
Yes 45.4% 42.1%  
In the past semester, did you watch any video or TV program on tobacco control on campus?
No or don't know 64.3% 67.7% 0.2465
Yes 35.7% 32.3%  
In the past semester, did anyone in your school teach you that smokers would have yellow teeth, wrinkles, or bad smell?
No or don't know 57.4% 61.1% 0.2282
Yes 42.6% 39.0%  
In the past semester, did anyone in your school teach you skills on how to refuse smoking?
No or don't know 69.8% 73.7% 0.1678
Yes 30.2% 26.3%  
Do you often see anti-smoking information on campus?
No 21.0% 34.4% <0.0001
Some times 48.5% 46.0%  
very often 30.5% 19.7%  

Table 1: Characteristics of the sampled students in junior high schools at Ningbo City, by smoking susceptibility in the next 12 months.

Table 2 presents results from the logistic regressions for boys. Model 1 examined the multivariate association between smoking-related knowledge and smoking susceptibility, controlling for residential area and grade in school. Compared with students from rural areas, urban students had a reduced risk of being susceptible to smoking in the next 12 months (OR=0.61; 95% CI: 0.41-0.91). And among seventh grade boys, the risk was 41% lower compared with their eighth grade counterparts (OR=0.59; 95% CI: 0.44-0.80). Boys who did not believe that smoking can cause lung cancer had an almost two-fold higher risk of being susceptible to smoking (OR=2.73; 95% CI: 1.59-4.70).

Students who believed that smoking could help people relax had over one-fold higher risk of smoking susceptibility (OR=2.32; 95% CI: 1.62-3.31). On the other hand, school boys who thought that boys who smoked were less attractive had a 36% lower risk of being susceptible to smoking (OR=0.64; 95% CI: 0.43-0.96). Whether girls who smoked were determined to be more attractive or less attractive had no impact on boys’ smoking susceptibility (Model 2).

Compared with having friends who were all or almost all smokers, having no smoking friends reduced a boy’s risk of smoking susceptibility by 78% (OR=0.22; 95% CI: 0.11-0.45), and having only some smoking friends reduced the risk by 52% (OR=0.48; 95% CI: 0.24-0.96) among boys (Model 3). Believing that their parents would mind their smoking when they grew up cut the boys’ risk of smoking susceptibility by half (OR=0.50; 95% CI: 0.34-0.75; Model 4).

Model 5 examined the influence of school environment on smoking susceptibility among boys. Compared with those who had seen teachers smoking in the past week, boys who had not seen teachers smoking had a 39% lower risk of smoking susceptibility (OR=0.61; 95% CI: 0.45-0.84). Moreover, boys who had not seen any anti-smoking information on campus had an almost a one-fold higher risk of being susceptible to smoking compared with those who had seen such information (OR=1.80; 95% CI: 1.16-2.79).

When all factors were included in the logistic regression (Model 6), the belief that smoking could help people relax predicted increased susceptibility to smoking (OR=1.89; 95% CI: 1.28-2.80), and having no classmates who were smokers greatly reduced the risk of susceptibility compared with having most of or all classmates who were smokers (OR=0.38; 95% CI: 0.18-0.79).

Table 3 presents results of the logistic regression for girls. The findings were similar to those for boys, in particular from analysis in which all types of risk factors were included simultaneously (Model 6). A significant difference between girls and boys in terms of associations between risk factors and susceptibility resulted from the question, “Do you think that girls who smoke are more attractive?”, which predicted smoking susceptibility for girls (OR=0.41; 95% CI: 0.22-0.77 in Model 6), but not for boys.

Discussion

This study aims to understand the risk factors of susceptibility to smoking among adolescents in the context of a developing country. Many findings of this study are consistent with evidence from developed countries. For example, previous studies found that beliefs about negative consequences for social desirability among adolescents dissuade young adults from smoking [12-14], while perceived positive reactions to smoking and normative beliefs about smoking among peers were found to increase adolescents’ risk of smoking susceptibility or initiation [10,15,16]. Similarly, we found that both boys and girls who believed that cigarettes’ had a relaxing effect were more susceptible to smoking (OR=1.89; 95% CI: 1.28-2.80 for boys; OR=2.37; 95% CI: 1.24-4.53 for girls). In addition, boys and girls differed on the subjective norms governing smokers’ image. Both groups appeared more concerned with images of their own gender than the opposite sex, which may suggest an emphasis on self-image. Boys perceiving that boys who smoked were less attractive had a 36% lower risk of being susceptible to smoking (OR=0.64; 95% CI: 0.43-0.96) in Model 2, although in the overall model (Model 6), this association was no longer significant at the .05 level. On the other hand, girls who believed that girls who smoked were more attractive had an over two-fold higher risk of smoking susceptibility, even after other factors had been controlled (OR=3.69; 95% CI: 1.33-10.25; Model 6). This finding suggested that girls appear more vulnerable to the stereotype of “coolness” around smoking [12,13].

Familial environments have been argued to play an important role in adolescent smoking initiation. Parents’ attitudes, perceptions, and expectations towards smoking (teaching), as well as parents’ own smoking behavior (modeling) have been found to influence adolescents’ smoking involvement [8,15,17-21]. The association, however, was sometimes found to be weak or inconsistent, whereas stronger evidence emerged on the influences of peers and older siblings [22-24], which was echoed by our findings. For example, we found that having no smokers as friends and classmates cut the risk of smoking susceptibility by around two-thirds for both boys and girls (OR=0.38; 95% CI: 0.18-0.79 for boys; OR=0.26; 95% CI: 0.07-0.96 for girls; Model 6), independent of familial effect. In contrast, believing that parents would have a problem with them smoking once grown cut the risk of smoking susceptibility by half among boys (OR=0.50; 95% CI: 0.34-0.75; Model 4); and not having anyone smoke in their presence at home or at school in a typical week also cut the risk by half among girls (OR=0.51; 95% CI: 0.30-0.86; Model 4). However, these effects were no longer statistically significant after peer factors were controlled for (Model 6). These findings were consistent with the literature that when presented simultaneously, neither the parental nor familial factors were predictive of smoking susceptibility, whereas peer influences remained strong [22-24]. Whether parental factors influence adolescent smoking susceptibility through moderating effects of peer factors merits future research. A previous study on 1,320 students from sixth to ninth grades found that parental involvement, monitoring, and expectations protected adolescents from smoking partially by limiting the number of friends who were smokers [21].

Characteristics Model 1-Knowledge Model 2- Attitude Model 3- Peer Model 4- Family Model 5- School Model 6- Overall
Area (Rural)            
Urban 0.61 (0.41-0.91) 0.52 (0.35-0.78) 0.84 (0.54-1.30) 0.58 (0.39-0.86) 0.67 (0.44-1.03) 0.88 (0.54-1.44)
Sub-urban 0.78 (0.50-1.22) 0.69 (0.44-1.08) 0.97 (0.60-1.57) 0.74 (0.48-1.15) 0.75 (0.47-1.18) 0.96 (0.57-1.63)
Grade (8th)            
7th 0.59 (0.44-0.80) 0.68 (0.50-0.92) 0.83 (0.60-1.15) 0.64 (0.48-0.86) 0.67 (0.49-0.91) 0.82 (0.58-1.16)
Do you think smoking can cause the following diseases:
Bronchitis (Yes)            
No 1.49 (0.99-2.23)         1.33 (0.84-2.09)
Hypertension(Yes)            
No 0.85 (0.58-1.25)         0.97 (0.63-1.51)
Lung cancer (Yes)            
No 2.73 (1.59-4.70)         1.79 (0.94-3.41)
heart attack (Yes)            
No 1.22 (0.82-1.81)         1.07 (0.69-1.67)
Stroke (Yes)            
No 0.91 (0.62-1.33)         0.95 (0.62-1.47)
Do you think smoking can help people relax? (No)            
Yes   2.32 (1.62-3.31)       1.89 (1.28-2.80)
Do you think smoking boys are more attractive? (No difference)            
More attractive   1.41 (0.79-2.53)       1.65 (0.87-3.13)
Less attractive   0.64 (0.43-0.96)       0.94 (0.60-1.47)
Do you think smoking girls are more attractive? (No difference)            
More attractive   1.61 (0.89-2.90)       0.92 (0.48-1.76)
Less attractive   0.87 (0.58-1.32)       0.75 (0.48-1.19)
As far as you know, does any of your friends or classmates smoke? (Most of or all)            
None     0.22 (0.11-0.45)     0.38 (0.18-0.79)
Some     0.48 (0.24-0.96)     0.62 (0.30-1.30)
Do you think your parent would not mind your smoking after you grow up? (Yes)            
No       0.50 (0.34-0.75)   0.95 (0.57-1.60)
In a typical week, how many days do people smoke in your presence at home? (1-7 days)            
None       0.78 (0.58-1.07)   1.11 (0.77-1.61)
In the past week, did you see your teachers smoking? (Yes)            
No         0.61 (0.45-0.84) 0.70 (0.48-1.01)
In the past semester, did the school distribute any tobacco control material to you? (Yes)            
No         0.82 (0.55-1.23) 0.90 (0.58-1.41)
In the past semester, did you watch any video or TV program on tobacco control on campus? (Yes)            
No         0.65 (0.41-1.03) 0.61 (0.36-1.03)
In the past semester, did anyone in your school teach you that smokers would have yellow teeth, wrinkles, or bad smell? (Yes)            
No         0.99 (0.67-1.46) 1.05 (0.69-1.62)
In the past semester, did anyone in your school teach you skills on how to refuse smoking? (Yes)            
No         1.09 (0.72-1.65) 1.12 (0.70-1.79)
Do you often see anti-smoking information on campus? (very often)            
None         1.80 (1.16-2.79) 1.16 (0.71-1.91)
Some times         1.43 (0.95-2.16) 1.13 (0.72-1.78)

Table 2: Estimated Odds Ratio (95 % confidence interval) of smoking susceptibility among boys.

Findings from our study also suggested that school interventions were largely ineffective at preventing student susceptibility to smoking, which is consistent with some existing literature. For example, a meta-analysis on smoking prevention programs revealed that programs focused on delivering knowledge about smoking stopped being effective one year post program, although those oriented towards behavioral change remained in effect over a period of three years [25]. Similarly, a review of adolescent smoking prevention programs in South Korea reported that the effectiveness of knowledge-based smoking prevention programs were lower than expected [26]. We are aware that most of the survey questions in our study referred to programs and campaigns that had taken place within the previous semester. A systematic review of school-based smoking prevention programs found that brevity is one of the main reasons of program failure and that for programs to have long-term practical effects, they must be carried out in at least fifteen sessions over several years, also continuing into high school [27].

Characteristics Model 1-Knowledge Model 2- Attitude Model 3- Peer Model 4- Family Model 5- School Model 6- Overall
Area (Rural)            
Urban 1.20 (0.55-2.61) 1.07 (0.49-2.33) 1.29 (0.58-2.89) 1.26 (0.58-2.70) 1.29 (0.58-2.88) 1.27 (0.52-3.13)
Sub-urban 1.33 (0.57-3.14) 1.27 (0.54-3.03) 1.56 (0.64-3.75) 1.34 (0.58-3.11) 1.34 (0.57-3.17) 1.38 (0.52-3.68)
Grade (8th)            
7th 0.31 (0.18-0.53) 0.35 (0.20-0.60) 0.47 (0.27-0.82) 0.34 (0.20-0.58) 0.32 (0.19-0.54) 0.41 (0.22-0.77)
Do you think smoking can cause the following diseases:
Bronchitis (Yes)            
No 1.39 (0.69-2.78)         1.12 (0.48-2.61)
Hypertension(Yes)            
No 0.64 (0.36-1.13)         0.73 (0.38-1.38)
Lung cancer (Yes)            
No 3.73 (1.69-8.24)         2.64 (0.94-7.45)
heart attack (Yes)            
No 1.23 (0.67-2.26)         1.00 (0.51-1.97)
Stroke (Yes)            
No 0.63 (0.36-1.10)         0.74 (0.39-1.41)
Do you think smoking can help people relax? (No)            
Yes   3.29 (1.88-5.78)       2.37 (1.24-4.53)
Do you think smoking boys are more attractive? (No difference)            
More attractive   0.51 (0.16-1.57)       0.48 (0.13-1.81)
Less attractive   0.49 (0.25-0.97)       0.63 (0.30-1.30)
Do you think smoking girls are more attractive? (No difference)            
More attractive   5.30(2.13-13.16)       3.69 (1.33-10.25)
Less attractive   1.06 (0.49-2.26)       1.03 (0.45-2.23)
As far as you know, does any of your friends or classmates smoke? (Most of or all)            
None     0.09 (0.03-0.27)     0.26 (0.07-0.96)
Some     0.33 (0.11-0.97)     0.67 (0.17-2.57)
Do you think your parents would not mind your smoking after you grow up? (Yes)            
No       0.51 (0.24-1.09)   0.79 (0.30-2.06)
In a typical week, how many days do people smoke in your presence at home? (1-7 days)            
None       0.51 (0.30-0.86)   0.63 (0.34-1.17)
In the past week, did you see your teachers smoking? (Yes)            
No            
In the past semester, did the school distribute any tobacco control material to you? (Yes)            
No         0.77 (0.41-1.46) 1.14 (0.53-2.44)
In the past semester, did you watch any video or TV program on tobacco control on campus? (Yes)            
No         1.13 (0.54-2.34) 1.12 (0.49-2.59)
In the past semester, did anyone in your school teach you that smokers would have yellow teeth, wrinkles, or bad smell? (Yes)            
No         0.92 (0.50-1.67) 0.89 (0.45-1.77)
In the past semester, did anyone in your school teach you skills on how to refuse smoking? (Yes)            
No         0.77 (0.41-1.44) 0.84 (0.42-1.69)
Do you often see anti-smoking information on campus? (very often)            
None         2.05 (1.01-4.15) 1.16 (0.51-2.66)
Some times         1.19 (0.63-2.23) 0.78 (0.39-1.56)

Table 3: Estimated Odds Ratio (95 % confidence interval) of smoking susceptibility among girls.

Caution should be exercised with these findings. First, the susceptibility to smoking was self-reported, and may be subjected to students’ reluctance to disclose their future intention of smoking [28], despite the appropriate methods, such as anonymity of questionnaires and absence of teachers from the classroom when the surveys were administered. Second, as a cross-sectional study, the findings from this study should not be considered causal. For example, the positive association between peer smoking behaviors and susceptibility to smoking of the respondent may not be due to peer effect, but rather to peer selection, being that youth who have not made a firm commitment not to smoke are more likely to have friends who are smokers.

Despite these limitations, the evidence from this case study suggested that susceptibility to smoking may be shaped by a complicated process, in which the interplay of many factors, including social norms, peer, family, and school, may play a role [1,29]. It may therefore be recommended that interventions aimed at curbing youth smoking initiation should be grounded in a comprehensive and long-lasting model, which may effectively change the psychological motivations underlying adolescent smoking intentions and behaviors [30,31], and build communication and decision-making skills, as well as assertiveness training to resist the negative influences of peers, family, and the community [1,11,29].

References

--
Post your comment

Share This Article

Article Usage

  • Total views: 15957
  • [From(publication date):
    specialissue-2013 - Oct 10, 2024]
  • Breakdown by view type
  • HTML page views : 11527
  • PDF downloads : 4430
Top