Dersleri yüzünden oldukça stresli bir ruh haline sikiş hikayeleri bürünüp özel matematik dersinden önce rahatlayabilmek için amatör pornolar kendisini yatak odasına kapatan genç adam telefonundan porno resimleri açtığı porno filmini keyifle seyir ederek yatağını mobil porno okşar ruh dinlendirici olduğunu iddia ettikleri özel sex resim bir masaj salonunda çalışan genç masör hem sağlık hem de huzur sikiş için gelip masaj yaptıracak olan kadını gördüğünde porn nutku tutulur tüm gün boyu seksi lezbiyenleri sikiş dikizleyerek onları en savunmasız anlarında fotoğraflayan azılı erkek lavaboya geçerek fotoğraflara bakıp koca yarağını keyifle okşamaya başlar

GET THE APP

International Journal of Emergency Mental Health and Human Resilience - Construction and Validation of the Contextual Victimization Questionnaire (CVCV) with Mexican Young Adults
ISSN: 1522-4821

International Journal of Emergency Mental Health and Human Resilience
Open Access

Our Group organises 3000+ Global Conferenceseries Events every year across USA, Europe & Asia with support from 1000 more scientific Societies and Publishes 700+ Open Access Journals which contains over 50000 eminent personalities, reputed scientists as editorial board members.

Open Access Journals gaining more Readers and Citations
700 Journals and 15,000,000 Readers Each Journal is getting 25,000+ Readers

This Readership is 10 times more when compared to other Subscription Journals (Source: Google Analytics)

Construction and Validation of the Contextual Victimization Questionnaire (CVCV) with Mexican Young Adults

Gloria Margarita Gurrola-Peña1*, Patricia Estado Balcázar-Nava1, Oscar Esparza Del Villar2, Georgina Lozano-Razo3 and Javier Zavala-Rayas3
1Autonomous University of the State of Mexico, Mexico
2Autonomous University of Ciudad Juárez, Mexico
3Autonomous University of Zacatecas, Mexico
*Corresponding Author: Gloria Margarita Gurrola-Peña, Mexico, Email: mgurrolaunid@hotmail.com

DOI: 10.4172/1522-4821.1000408

Keywords: Community violence; Victimization; Youths; Validity; Reliability

Introduction

The World Health Organization (WHO; cited by Organización Panamericana de la Salud, OPS; 2002) defines violence as: The deliberate use of physical force or strength whether in a threatening or effective manner, against oneself, other persons, a group or a community, which causes or is likely to cause injury, death, psychological damage, developmental disorders or deprivation (p. 4).

In this respect, despite the fact that information is scarce because the quantity and quality of data is deficient worldwide, it is estimated that 1.6 million people died violently in the year 2000, with young people aged 15 to 24 being the most affected (Krug, Dahlberg, Mercy, Zwi & Lozano, 2002; WHO, cited by OPS, 2002). Despite the fact that the rates of violence in 2012 show a reduction of 16% worldwide (WHO, 2014) it is still considered a serious public health problem since it affects children, adolescents, young adults, adults and the elderly. Given the diversity of violent acts, for their analysis one should focus on the common features and relationships between the various types of violence, the following classification is proposed depending on the perpetrator of the violent act (WHO, cited by OPS; 2002):

• Self-inflicted violence: Includes suicidal behavior and selfharm.

• Collective violence: Subdivided into social violence (mass violence, terrorism and collective acts of hatred, political violence (wars) and economic violence (group looting for economic profit).

• Interpersonal violence: Subdivided into two subcategories, first, family or partner violence, which is usually occur, but not always, at home. Secondly, community violence that refers to violence that occurs between people who are not relatives and who may or may not know each other and usually happens outside the home, therefore does not include domestic violence or verbal abuse.

Specifically, community violence encompasses all types of crime (assault, rape, robbery, kidnapping, drugs, exposure to firearms, knives and homicides) (Kennedy and Ceballo, 2014; Krug et al., 2002), and although it can be a by-product of different circumstances, it is characterized by being developed in public spaces closest to the people subjected to them. The above is supported by Scarpa, Haden & Hurley (2006) when they affirm that this type of violence takes place close to home, school and neighborhood. In this regard, at the onset of adulthood and due to the processes of socialization and independence, young people usually spend more time outside the home and more time in the street, which is why they are exposed more frequently to community violence, oftentimes becoming victims. On the other hand, given its high incidence, community violence today has been considered as something every day, a routine fact with which one has learned to live and that only impacts when one is a direct victim or when by its magnitude and seriousness it causes visible damage. This phenomenon has a twofold effect that has been overlooked: Individually when it affects the quality of life and collectively when it influences community development. This is due to the fact that people who have been direct victims transmit their experiences to others. This creates a far-reaching, all-encompassing fear that leads the community to think they are potential victims and as such, they become contextual victims. This is why greater precision is necessary for the different forms of exposure. According to Echeburúa (2004), the types of victims or those affected are the following:

• Direct victims or primary sufferers: People directly affected by the aggression or the traumatic event.

• Secondary or indirect victims. Included in this category are family members and people that are close to the direct victims: those who are traumatized by the physical and socio-cultural conditions after directly witnessing the violence.

• Contextual victims or affected: Those who are traumatized by the physical and socio-cultural conditions of their violent communities, this category includes people who have been psychologically affected by serious events without suffering any direct losses or threats to their lives or their family.

Taking into account the definitions of community violence and contextual victimization, we can indicate that there is a third category of exposure to community violence different from direct victim and witness named contextual victim of community violence. This type of victimization is characterized by hearing about violence in the public environments closest to people. Victimization has become an urgent concern for individuals, families and communities and there has been a growing interest in monitoring the patterns and impact of community violence exposure and this has given rise to the creation of diverse scales to measure the phenomenon from the victim’s standpoint, especially children and adolescents. Thus, while the research literature in children and adolescents is increasing, there is little research on exposure to community violence in young adults, adults and the elderly (De Cou & Lynch, 2015).

In this sense, when analyzing the main questionnaires (for a more exhaustive list see De Cou & Lynch, 2015) used to evaluate exposure to community violence in adults, it was found that most questionnaires were designed for children and teenagers. Reviewing various databases (ProQuest Social Science, PRISMA, PsycINFO) to identify the questionnaires used to assess exposure, community violence in young adults was found to the Survey of Exposure to Community Violence (SECV) (Richters & Saltzman, 1990), is notably the most widely used to assess exposure to community violence in teens and adults, and includes victimization reagents at home, school or neighborhood (i.e., persecutions, beatings or shootings, stabbings, murders or rapes). The scale used on adults reports an internal consistency that ranges from acceptable (α =0.74) to excellent (α = 0.85). This survey to include items that explore exposure in the home does not meet the characteristics of happening in public spaces outside the home and does not explore the exposure through hearing about violence (Foege, Rosenberg and Mercy, 1995; WHO, 2002). Cooley, Turner and Beidel (1995) designed and published the Childrens Report of Exposure to Violence (CREV) aimed at young people from 9 to 18 years, which consists of 29 items that measure exposure through victimization, direct observation, those reported by others and, unlike the previous ones, includes direct observation on television. The questionnaire reports an acceptable internal consistency (α = 0.78). The testretest reliability was 0.75 for the Total score (p<.001). This scale includes exposure through direct exposure on television, however this form of exposure could not be considered direct or community. The Screen for Adolescent Violence Exposure (SAVE) (Hastings & Kelley, 1997) is a self-reporting scale consisting of 32 items assessing frequent exposure to community violence in three different areas: school, neighborhood and home. This scale includes direct exposure to life-threatening situations, it also includes having heard that others close to them had experienced life-threatening situation and finally, having experienced or observed violent or aggressive behaviors that were not lifethreatening (i.e., physical or verbal abuse). The scale shows excellent internal consistency indexes (school α = 0.93, home α = 0.94 and neighborhood α = 0.95). The authors also report the results of the confirmatory factor analysis where a three-factor model is reported (school, neighborhood and home). The model suggests an appropriate adjustment: χ2 = 722.96, p<.001, CFI = 0.90. This questionnaire, although it includes listening to community violence, does not treat it as a different category since it includes it in the category of witness. Likewise, an adjustment of the scale of exposure to community violence was made (Richters and Saltzman, 1990) reducing it to 18 items divided into two subscales: one that reflects the degree of exposure to community violence as a direct victim during high school years and one that reflects exposure to community violence as a witness during the same period. (Rosenthal, 2000, Rosenthal & Wilson, 2003; 2006). With respect to the latter, instructions specify that youngsters should not include things they have seen on television, heard on the radio, read in newspapers or witnessed at home. This adjustment shows a good internal consistency (α =0.88). This scale does not explore the exposure through hearing about community violence which is an essential feature in contextual victimization (Echeburúa, 2004). Another adaptation of the SECV was designed by Schwartz and Proctor (2000) who created the Community Violence Exposure (CVE) whose 25 items focus on events that occur in public places (i.e., work, school) and exclude acts that occur at home or those committed by family members. The scale has a good internal consistency (α = 0.88). This adaptation, despite circumscribing community violence in the public sphere, leaves aside the fact of hearing about violence as a form of exposure. Orue and Calvete (2010) developed the Violence Exposure Questionnaire (CEV) which consists of 21 items that measure direct and indirect victimization in four contexts: school, street, home and TV. The Cronbach coefficients were 0.73, 0.78, 0.71 and 0.77 respectively for the observation of violence in the school, the neighborhood, the home and the TV, and 0.79, 0.75 and 0.80 for the victimization in the school, the neighborhood and at home, in the case of the exhibition to the TV violence, there are no items referring to victimization. The confirmatory factor analysis showed a good fit, χ2 = 183, p<.001, RMSEA = 0.067, NNFI = 0.92, CFI = 0.93. This questionnaire includes items that explore the exhibition in the home which is not a public place and the exhibition through television which is not considered a community. As it can be seen, there are different measures to evaluate exposure to community violence, most of which are more than ten years old and, except for the one designed by Orue y Calvete (2010), there is only the English version. Another element to highlight is that they are based on different definitions of community violence. Some authors use the term community as a synonym of neighborhood, excluding family and TV, and others include violence at home, school and TV (Guerra, Huesmann & Spindler, 2003). Guterman, Cameron & Staller (2000), recommend two things for studies of community violence: the first is that the definition should include the meaning of community, and in the case of this study, public environments closest to the people outside the home; and secondly, it must show the type of violence, including domestic, partner and school (Aisenberg & Herrenkohl, 2008; De Cou & Lynch, 2015), and, in this case, all types of crimes (Forge et al, 1995; Fowler et al, 2009; Kennedy & Ceballo, 2014; Krug et al., 2002; Scarpa et al, 2006; WHO, cited by OPS, 2002). On the other hand, researchers in this area have conceptualized exposure by being an eye witness and exposure through being a direct victim as separate constructs given their different consequences. In general, the questionnaires for exposure to community violence include questions about whether the subject witnessed or was a victim of violence. They also normally have questions of what is usually called “indirect exposure” which includes having a close relative who has been a victim when the person was absent (Hamby & Finkhelor, 2001), therefore, personal victimization and witnessing violence are the two most explored categories and have been clearly defined. Eitle & Turner (2002), Scarpa, Hurley, Shumate & Haden (2006), Brennan, Molnar & Earls (2007), Elsaesser, Sung & Voisin (2016), introduce a third exposure study category which consists of hearing about acts of community violence, arguing that the mere fact of hearing about violence very close to home has negative psychological impacts on persons and the above corresponds to the contextual victimization described by Aisenberg & Herrenkohl, 2008; Kennedy & Ceballo, 2014; De Cou & Lynch, 2015. Thus, in their study, Eitle & Turner (2002) used 5 questions asking about having witnessed violence or hearing about violent crimes in their community, which can predict mental health problems in young adults (i.e. They were told someone they knew had been shot) last year and throughout their lifetime; the authors do not show the reliability of the subscale. Scarpa et al. (2006) used 14 items from the Survey of Exposure to Community Violence (Richters and Saltzman, 1990) that explore having heard about acts of violence in their communities and the subscale shows a good internal consistency (α=0.86). Brennan et al. (2007), after showing empirically that there are three routes of exposure to community violence (victimization, witnessing and hearing about violence) propose a reduced version of their scale of exposure to violence that consists of 10 items that explore direct victimization, 10 items that explore being a witness and 5 items that explore having heard about violence; the same authors mention that the small number of responses affect internal consistency, which is evident in the subscale hearing about violence (α = 0.50) so they propose to add more items in order to have an questionnaire to research the subject. Finally in their study, Elsaesser et al. (2016), include three responses on hearing about acts of violence in the community, and the subscale shows a deficient internal consistency (α = 0.74). As seen, there are three possible forms of being exposed to community violence, as a direct victim, as an indirect victim or witness, and as an affected or contextual victim that, although in the context of exposure to community violence are strongly interrelated and they belong to the same construct, contextual victimization in particular requires more research in order to evaluate its future impact on mental health. This is because despite the fact that the few studies that have examined the influence of this type of violence indicate an adverse impact on young people’s mental health, most studies ignore the influence of this third type of exposure to community violence (Elsaesser, 2018).Due to the above, this study intends to fill the literary gap by clearly defining what is considered community violence, addressing an understudied population such as young adults and exploring a rarely studied phenomenon such as contextual victimization, therefore, the goal of the present study was to design and validate a questionnaire for young adults on contextual victimization of community violence.

Methods

Participants

The present study included two samples. The first was to build and explore the internal consistency of the contextual victimization questionnaire in cases of community violence using 2,289 volunteer students from 11 universities in different regions of Mexico, 18 to 25 years old with an average of 20 (S.D. = 2.1) years, of which 931 were women and 1,358 were men. The second sample was designed to carry out the questionnaire’s confirmatory factorial analysis using 1,507 volunteer students from four universities in the northern and central regions of Mexico, between 18 and 25 years with an average of 21 (S.D. = 2.2) years, of which 995 were women and 512 men. All participants were volunteer students with diverse careers, Mexican, Spanish-speaking and of all socioeconomic status (high level 7.2%, medium-high level 14%, typical and emerging middle level typical 53.8%, low level 25%) and residents in states of the republic which are considered violent.

Measures

For this study, the Contextual Victimization of Community Violence (CVCV) questionnaire was specifically designed in Spanish, taking into account the stages for developing the measuring tools (Muñiz & Fonseca-Pedrero, 2008). Regarding the content of the items once the variables of community violence and contextual victimization were operationalized the National Survey of Victimization and Perception of Public Safety (Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía, 2016) was used to consult the main crimes committed in the communities the students belonged to and ask about them. Examples of exposure to crimes were theft, armed robberies, shootings, kidnappings, murders. In terms of format, the self-reporting questionnaire was selected following the recommendations of Hamby and Finkhelor (2001), who recommended this because victimization is a sensitive subject and victims are reluctant to speak on their own behalf; additionally, due to its simplicity and ease of use, self-reporting allows for evaluating a large number of people simultaneously, is low-cost, unobtrusive, and may be done anonymously.

Because few questionnaires have been given to young adults, this population was chosen because they participate in more activities away from home due to their ages and therefore, may be more exposed to community violence committed by strangers. Two procedures were used to verify the validity of the contents and the applicability of the first version: (a) five experts were asked to participate and analyze the effectiveness of the items according to theoretical dimensions and to eliminate confusion; (b) a pilot study was done using a sample of 200 young university students in order to eliminate items that were unclear.

The final version was a questionnaire with 40 line items with five response options ranging from “never happened” to “very frequently happened to me in the last year”.

Procedure

After obtaining the approval of the respective ethics commissions, the researchers proceeded to request student volunteer participation who, after signing an informed consent document, answered the questionnaires which were written in Spanish used in Mexico. The paper and pencil questionnaires were given collectively in the various classes the students were attending and filling it out took 20 to 30 minutes. The confidentiality of the data was ensured by assigning a folio to each completed questionnaire without including the student’s name.

Results

The results of the exploratory factor analysis indicate that the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measurement of sampling adequacy was .94 and the Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity was 17,367.86 (p = <.001) which provides additional evidence that the correlation matrix was adequate for the factor analysis. When the Exploratory Factorial Analysis (EFA) was carried out, 6 items were eliminated because they shared factorial loads in two factors. Of the remaining 34 items, 5 factors with eigenvalues greater than one were extracted. When the EFA was performed, 6 items were eliminated due to shared-load factors in two factors; of the 34 remaining items, 5 factors with eigenvalues greater than one were removed. The items of the extracted factors show factorial loads that are in a range of .50 to .81; the total of the variance explained by the 5 factors was 59.52%, with a .94 Cronbach alpha. The first factor contains items regarding hearing about various types of crimes committed in any community setting (i.e. I have heard that in places where I go to have fun, someone was shot). This factor explains 39.10% of the total variance and contains eight items that evaluate nonwitness contextual victimization with 90 alpha. The second factor contains items about having been an eyewitness or witnessed various types of crimes in any community setting (i.e. I have seen how drugs are trafficked in places I hang out) and with an alpha of .85 explains 7.28% of the total variance and with seven items representing having witnessed contextual victimization (witnessing community violence against strangers). The third factor contains items regarding hearing about the perpetration of various types of crimes in the youth’s neighborhood (i.e. I heard that someone was kidnapped in my neighborhood). Contains eight items related to contextual victimization in the neighborhood and explains 5.45% of the total variance with an alpha of .88. The fourth factor contains items refer to having heard about criminal acts in entertainment venues that young people frequent (i.e. I’ve heard that in places I go often, assaults without weapons are carried out) with an alpha of .86 explains 4.64% of the total variance. Finally, the fifth factor contains items about having heard of various types of crimes committed at the schools young people attend (i.e. I have heard that someone in my school has been shot).explains 3.04% of the total variance and contains five items that evaluate contextual victimization in school with an alpha of .80. To confirm the factor structure of the Contextual Victimization by Community Violence Questionnaire we performed a Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA). The model includes four factors with nine items for Factor 1, labeled General contextual victimization; six items for Factor 2, labeled Contextual victimization in the neighborhood; six items for Factor 3, labeled Contextual victimization in recreational areas; and 4 items for Factor 4, labeled Contextual victimization in schools (Table 1). Each item on the scale has a single weight for each factor. For this analysis, we chose the item with the highest weight factor per factor and its variance was predetermined to be equal to 1. The values suggested by Hooper, Coughlan & Mullen (2008) for Structural Equation Models were used to evaluate the goodness of fit: Adjusted Goodness of Fit Statistic (AGFI) ≥ .90, Normed-Fit Index (NFI) ≥ .90, Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) ≤ .06, Comparative Fit Index (CFI) ≥ .90. Hu and Bentler (1999) suggested using these indexes to evaluate the model’s goodness of fit since the Chi-Square Test can easily be statistically significant if the sample is large or the data is abnormal. The present model yielded the following results: χ2 (266) = 1738.89 (p<.01), AGFI = .90, NFI = .91, CFI = .92, RMSEA = .06. Apart from the value of Chi-Square, other indexes show a positive goodness of fit. Factor weights ranged between .59 and .83 (Table 1). The internal consistency of each one of the factors was evaluated using Cronbach’s alpha. The result of the alpha indexes for the entire scale and each factor are: α = .92 for item totals, α = .91 for Factor 1, α = .88 for Factor 2, α = .82 for Factor 3, and α = .74 for Factor 4. Finally, the correlation between each of the factors on the Contextual Victimization by Community Violence Questionnaire was performed (Table 2). All the correlations between the factors were statistically significant and ranged from r = .27 (moderate) to r = .54 (strong).

Factor Weights
Items Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4
1. I have heard that in the places I go for fun, people have been killed 0.69      
3. I have heard that in the places I go for fun, someone has been shot 0.74      
4. I have heard that in the places I go for fun, people have been stabbed 0.7      
5. I have heard that people have been hurt in the places I go for fun 0.75      
10. I have heard that in the places I often go, people have been stabbed 0.64      
12. I have heard that in the places I often go, someone has been shot 0.75      
16. I have heard that in the places I often go, people have been killed 0.77      
24. I have heard that people have been hurt in the places I often go 0.75      
26. I have heard that in the places I go for fun, someone has been kidnapped 0.67      
15. I have heard that in my neighborhood, someone has been kidnapped   0.67    
18. I have heard that drugs are sold in my neighborhood   0.64    
19. I have heard that in my neighborhood, someone has been shot   0.83    
20. I have heard that in my neighborhood, people have been beaten up   0.74    
28. I have heard that in my neighborhood, people have been killed   0.76    
34. I have heard that people have been hurt in my neighborhood   0.78    
14. I have heard that people have been attacked with weapons in the places I often go     0.64  
21. I have heard that in the places I often go, someone has been stabbed     0.6  
22. I have heard that there have been unarmed attacks in the places I go for fun     0.59  
27. I have heard that someone has been kidnapped in the places I often go     0.7  
30. I have heard that in the places I often go, someone has been shot     0.73  
32. I have heard that people have been attacked with weapons in the places I often go     0.64  
6. I have heard that at my school, people have been beaten up       0.64
7. I have heard that at my school, someone has been shot       0.63
8. I have heard that people have been stabbed at my school       0.66
13. I have heard that people have been hurt at my school.       0.68

Table 1: Completely standardized factor weights for confirmatory factor analysis

Factors Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4
Factor 1        
Factor 2 0.54**      
Factor 3 0.52** 0.40**    
Factor 4 0.41** 0.27** 0.49**  
** p<.01

Table 2: Correlation of factors on the conextual victimization scale

Discussion

This study’s most relevant contribution was the creation of a specific instrument to measure the frequency of contextual victimization of community violence in young adults, which makes it different from questionnaires like Richters & Saltzman, 1990; Cooley et al., 1995; Hastings & Kelley, 1997; Rosenthal, 2000; Rosenthal & Wilson, 2003; 2006, Schwartz and Proctor, 2000; Orue and Calvete, 2010; that although they have been used in adult studies they were originally designed for children and adolescents . Likewise, allowed in the first place clarify the construct of community violence in the sense that it is where criminal acts occur in the closest surroundings, but are outside people’s homes (Forge, Rosenberg & Mercy, 1995; Scarpa et al., 2006), namely, the neighborhood, recreational places and schools. Second, it showed evidence that contextual victimization exists, which consists of hearing about violent events that occurred in public environments closest to individuals (Brennan et al., 2007; Eitle & Turner, 2002; Scarpa et al., 2006). The above could be verified when performing the CFA, the model leaves out one of the subscales called face-toface victimization that emerged in the EFA and whose items would correspond to the witness category since it contained questions about having witnessed robberies, kidnappings, drug trafficking and beatings among other violent crimes. The results obtained revealed the appropriate psychometric properties of the items and the reliability and validity of the Contextual Victimization by Community Violence (CVCV) Questionnaire scores. The analyses provide evidence of the document structure, with four unifactorial sub-scales that are consistent with theoretical substantiations; also worthy of mentioning is the questionnaire’s overall satisfactory internal consistency index and its sub-scales. The CVCV Questionnaire shows advantages over others that are used to measure contextual violence (Brennan et al., 2007; Eitle & Turner, 2002; Elsaesser et al., 2016; Scarpa et al., 2006), first because it shows better internal consistency indexes, and second, because it is a wide questionnaire, allows confirmatory factor analysis to confirm the operationalization of the construct to be studied (Herrero, 2010). The VCVC questionnaire is of particular relevance since it will allow to distinguish in the future the differential impact of the types of victimization of community violence since most of the studies include victimization through being a witness and the contextual victimization (hearing reports about violence) in the category of indirect victimization. Contextual victimization (listening to reports of violence (Javdani, Abdul-Adil, Suárez, Nichols & Farmer, 2014, Lambert, Nylund-Gibson, Copeland- Linder & Ialongo, 2010). This is how the CVCV questionnaire when applied with other mental health questionnaires will be able to perform a more targeted analysis on the presentation of trauma and other symptoms in youths who have witnessed acts of violence and those who have only heard about the occurrence of these acts. The CVCV Questionnaire is of particular relevance since it will allow when applied with other mental health questionnaires to distinguish in the future the differential impact of the types of victimization of community violence since most of the studies include victimization through being a witness and the contextual victimization (hearing reports about violence) in the category of indirect victimization (Javdani, Abdul-Adil, Suárez, Nichols & Farmer, 2014, Lambert, Nylund-Gibson, Copeland-Linder & Ialongo, 2010). This is how the CVCV Questionnaire will be able to perform a more targeted analysis on the presentation of trauma and other symptoms in youths who have witnessed acts of violence and those who have only heard about the occurrence of these acts. This will allow providing evidence-based information on this type of victims to mental health centers while promoting the need for citizen security programs. The present study had some limitations that must be kept in mind for future research. First, when constructing the scale it was decided to explore the frequency of victimization during the previous year and that it would be particularly relevant to also explore victimization over a lifetime as this would identify many more victimization exposures. In this regard we know that people who experienced multiple or accumulated victimization have a higher risk of having negative impact on mental health and academic performance in young people (Gardella, Tanner-Smith & Fisher, 2016). Likewise, it should not be overlooked that the various criminal events do not have the same impact on youth, and it is for this reason that a range of possibilities for future studies is opened where researches are built to explore the seriousness of the events in the context of contextual victimization and its differential impact on mental health.

References

  1. Aisenberg, E., & Herrenkohl, T. (2008). Community violence in contex: risk and resilience in children and families. J Interpers Violence, 23(3): 296-315. Brennan, R.T., Molnar, B., & Earls, F. (2007). Refining the measurement of exposure to violence (ETV). J Community Psychol, 35(5): 603-618. Cooley, M.R., Turner, S.M., & Beidel, D.C. (1995). Assessing community violence: the children´s report of exposure to violence. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 34(2): 201-208. De Cou, C.R., & Lynch, S.M. (2015). Assesing adult exposure to community violence: A review of definition and measures. Trauma Violence Abuse, 18(1): 52-61. Echeburúa, E. (2004). Superar un trauma. Tratamiento de las víctimas de delitos violentos. Madrid: Pirámide. Eitle, D., & Turner, R.J. (2002). Exposure to community violence and young adult crime: The effects of witnessing violence, traumatic victimization, and other stressful life events. J. Res. Crime Delinq, 39(2): 214-237. Elsaesser, C. (2018). The longitudinal relations between dimensions of community violence exposure and developmental outcomes among adolescent ethnic minority males. Psychol Violence, 8(4): 409-417. Elsaesser, C., Hong, J. S., & Voisin, D. R. (2016). Violence exposure and bullying among African American adolscentes: Examining the protective role of academic engagement. Child Youth Serv Rev, 70(C): 394-402 Forge, W. H., Rosenberg, N. M., & Mercy, J. A. (1995). Public health and violence prevention. Curr Issues Public Health, 1: 2-9. Gardella, J. H., Tanner-Smith, E. E., & Fisher, B. W. (2016). Academic consequences of multiple victimization and the role of school security measures. Am J Community Psychol, 58(1-2), 36-46. Guerra, N.G., Huesmann, R., & Spindler, A. (2003). Community violence exposure, social cognition, and aggression among urban elementary school children. Child Dev, 74(5): 1561-1576. Guterman, N.B., Cameron, M., & Staller, K. (2000). Definitional and measurement issues in the study for community violence among children and youths. J Community Psychol, 28(6): 571-587. Hamby, S.L., & Finkerhor, D. (2001).Choosing and using child victimization questionnaries. Wachington, D. C., U.S. Departament of Justice, Office of Juenile Delinquency and Prevention. Hastings, T.L., & Kelley, M.L. (1997). Development and validation of the screen for adolescent violence exposure. J Abnorm Child Psychol, 25(6): 511-520. Herrero, J. (2010). Confirmatory factor analysis in the study of the structure and stability pf assesment instruments: An example with the self-esteem questionnaire (CA-14). Interv Psicosoc, 19(3). Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., & Mullen, M. R. (2008). Structural equation modelling: Guidelines for determining model fit. Electronic J Bus Res, 6(1): 53-60. Hu, L., & Bentler, P.M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling, 3: 424-453. Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía (2017). Encuesta Nacional sobre la Dinámica de las Relaciones en los Hogares (ENDIREH): México. Javdani, S., Abdul-Adil, J., Suárez, L., Nichols, S.R., & Farmer, A. D. (2014). Gender differences in the effects of community violence on mental health outcomes in a sample of low-income youth receiving psychiatric care. Am J Community Psychol, 53(3-4): 235-248. Kennedy, T.M., & Ceballo, R. (2014). Who, what, when and where? Toward a dimensional conceptualization of community violence exposure. Rev Gen Psychol, 18(2), 69-81. Krug, E.G., Dahlberg, L.L., Mercy, J. A., Zwi, A.B., & Lozano, R. (2002). World Report on Violence and Health. Geneva: World Health Organization. Lambert, S.F., NylundGibson, K., Copeland-Linder, N., & Lalongo, N.S. (2010). Patterns of community violence exposure during adolescence. Am J Community Psychol, 46(3-4): 289-302. Muñiz, J., & Fonseca-Pedrero, E. (2008). Construction of measurement systems for University Evaluation. J Res Educ, 5:13-25. Organización Panamericana de la Salud (2002). Informe Mundial sobre la Violencia y Salud. Washington, D.C.: Organización Panamericana de la Salud, Oficina Regional para las Américas. Orue, I., & Calvete, E. (2010). Elaboración y validación de un cuestionario para medir la exposición a la violencia en infancia y adolescencia. Int J Psychol Psicol Ter,10(2): 279-292. Ritchers, J.E., & Saltzman, W. (1990). Survey of Exposure to Community Violence Self (SECV) Report Version. Maryland: National Institute of Mental Health. Rosenthal, B.S (2000). Exposure to community violence in adolescence: trauma symptoms. Adolescence, 35(138): 271-284. Rosenthal, B.S., & Wilson, W.C. (2003). Impact of exposure to community violence and psychological symptoms on college performance among students of color. Adolescence, 38(150): 239-249. Rosenthal, B.S., & Wilson, W.C. (2006). Mental health services: use and disparity among diverse college students. J Am Coll Health, 57(1), 61-68. Scarpa, A., Hurley, J.D., Shumate, H.W., & Haden, S.C. (2006). Lifetime prevalence and socioemotional effects on hearing about community violence. J Interpers Violence, 21(1), 5-23. Scarpa, A., Haden, S.C., & Hurley, J. (2006). Community violence victimization and symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder: the moderating effects of coping and social support. J Interpers Violence, 21(4), 446-469. Schwartz, D., & Proctor, L.J. (2000). Community violence exposure and children´s social adjustment in the school peer group: the mediating roles of emotion regulation and social cognition. J Consult Clin Psychol, 68(4): 670-683. World Health Organization (2013). Global and regional estimates of violence against women: prevalence and health effects of intimate partner violence and non-partner sexual violence. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization. World Health Organization (2014). Global status report on violence prevention 2014. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.
Top